SSブログ

『意識の探求』の参考文献80-92

参考文献80
• Tamura, H. and Tanaka, K. “Visual response properties of cells in
the ventral and dorsal parts of the macque inferotemporal cortex,”
Cerebral Cortex 11:384–399 (2001).
• Tanaka, K. “Inferotemporal cortex and object vision,” Ann. Rev.
Neurosci. 19:109–139 (1996).
• Tanaka, K. “Columnar organization in the inferotemporal cortex.” In:
Cerebral Cortex. Vol. 12. Rockland, K.S., Kaas, J.H., and Peters, A.,
eds., pp. 469–498. New York: Plenum Press (1997).
• Tanaka, K. “Columns for complex visual object features in the inferotemporal
cortex: Clustering of cells with similar but slightly different
stimulus selectivities,” Cerebral Cortex 13:90–99 (2003).
• Tang, S. and Guo, A. “Choice behavior of Drosophila facing contradictory
visual cues,” Science 294:1543–1547 (2001).
• Tang, Y.-P., Shimizu, E., Dube, G.R., Rampon, C., Kerchner, G.A.,
Zhuo, M., Liu, G., and Tsien, J.Z. “Genetic enhancement of learning
and memory in mice,” Nature 401:63–69 (1999).
• Taylor, J.G. The Race for Consciousness. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press
(1998).
• Taylor, J.L. and McCloskey, D.I. “Triggering of preprogrammedmovements
as reactions to masked stimuli,” J. Neurophysiol. 63:439–444
(1990).
• Teller, D.Y. “Linking propositions,” Vision Res. 24:1233–1246 (1984).
• Teller, D.Y. and Pugh, E.N. Jr. “Linking propositions in color vision.”
In: Color Vision: Physiology and Psychophysics. Mollon, J.D. and
Sharpe, L.T., eds., London: Academic Press (1983).
• Thiele, A., Henning, P., Kubschik, M., and Hoffmann, K.-P. “Neural
mechanisms of saccadic suppression,” Science 295:2460–2462 (2002).
• Thiele, A. and Stoner, G. “Neuronal synchrony does not correlate with
motion coherence in cortical area MT,” Nature 23:366–370 (2003).
• Thier P., Haarmeier, T., Treue, S., and Barash, S. “Absence of a
common functional denominator of visual disturbance in cerebellar
disease,” Brain 122:2133–2146 (1999).
参考文献81
• Thomas, O.M., Cumming, B.G., and Parker, A.J. “A specialization
for relative disparity in V2,” Nature Neurosci. 5:472–478 (2002).
• Thompson, K.G. and Schall, J.D. “The detection of visual signals by
macaque frontal eye field during masking,” Nature Neurosci. 2:283–
288 (1999).
• Thompson, K.G., and Schall, J.D. “Antecedents and correlates of visual
detection and awareness in macaque prefrontal cortex,” Vision
Res. 40:1523–1538 (2000).
• Thorpe, S., Fize, D., and Marlot, C. “Speed of processing in the human
visual system,” Nature 381:520–522 (1996).
• Tolias, A.S., Smirnakis, S.M., Augath, M.A., Trinath, T., and Logothetis,
N.K. “Motion processing in the macaque: Revisited with
functional magnetic resonance imaging,” J. Neurosci. 21:8594–8601
(2001).
• Tomita, H., Ohbayashi,M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I., andMiyashita,
Y. “Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of
memory retrieval,” Nature 401:699–703 (1999).
• Tong, F. and Engel, S.A. “Interocular rivalry revealed in the human
cortical blind-spot representation,” Nature 411:195–199 (2001).
• Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Vaughan, J.T., and Kanwisher, N. “Binocular
rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex,” Neuron
21:753–759 (1998).
• Tong, F., Nakayama, K., Moscovitch,M.,Weinrib, O., and Kanwisher,
N. “Response properties of the human fusiform face area,” Cogn. Neuropsychol.
17:257–279 (2000).
• Tononi, G. and Edelman, G.M. “Consciousness and complexity,” Science
282:1846–1851 (1998).
• Tootell, R.B. and Hadjikhani, N. “Where is ‘dorsal V4’ in human
visual cortex? Retinotopic, topographic, and functional evidence,”
Cerebral Cortex 11:298–311 (2001).
• Tootell, R.B., Hadjikhani, N., Mendola, J.D., Marrett, S., and Dale,
A.M. “From retinotopy to recognition: Functional MRI in human
visual cortex,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 2:174–183 (1998).
参考文献82
• Tootell, R.B., Mendola, J.D., Hadjikhani, N., Ledden, P.J., Liu, A.K.,
Reppas, J.B., Sereno, M.I., and Dale, A.M. “Functional analysis of
V3A and related areas in human visual cortex,” J. Neurosci. 17:7060–
7078 (1997).
• Tootell, R.B., Reppas, J.B., Dale, A.M., Look, R.B., Sereno, M.I.,
Malach, R., Brady, T.J., and Rosen, B.R. “Visual motion aftereffect
in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance
imaging,” Nature 375:139–141 (1995).
• Tootell, R.B. and Taylor, J.B. “Anatomical evidence for MT and additional
cortical visual areas in humans,” Cerebral Cortex 5:39–55
(1995).
• Tranel, D. and Damasio, A.R. “Knowledge without awareness: An
autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics,” Science
228:1453–1454 (1985).
• Treisman, A. “Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial
Lecture,” Quart. J. Exp. Psychology 40A:201–237 (1988).
• Treisman, A. “The binding problem,” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 6:171–
178 (1996).
• Treisman, A. “Feature binding, attention and object perception,”
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 353:1295–1306 (1998).
• Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. “A feature-integration theory of attention,”
Cogn. Psychol. 12:97–136 (1980).
• Treisman, A. and Schmidt, H. “Illusory conjunctions in the perception
of objects,” Cogn. Psychol. 14:107–141 (1982).
• Treue, S. and Martinez-Trujillo, J.C. “Feature-based attention influences
motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex,” Nature
399:575–578 (1999).
• Treue, S. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Attentional modulation of visual motion
processing in cortical areas MT and MST,” Nature 382:539–541
(1996).
• Tsal, Y. “Do illusory conjunctions support feature integration theory?
A critical review of theory and findings,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Percept. Perform. 15:394–400 (1989).
参考文献83
• Tsotsos, J.K. “Analyzing vision at the complexity level,” Behav.Brain
Sci. 13:423–469 (1990).
• Tsunoda, K., Yamane, Y., Nishizaki, M., and Tanifuji, M. “Complex
objects represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination
of feature columns,” Nature Neurosci. 4:832–838 (2001).
• Tully, T. “Toward a molecular biology of memory: The light’s coming
on!,” Nature Neurosci. 1:543–545 (1998).
• Tully, T. and Quinn, W.G. “Classical conditioning and retention in
normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster,” J. Comp. Physiol. A
157:263–277 (1985).
• Tulunay-Keesey, ¨U. “Fading of stabilized retina images,” J. Opt. Soc.
Am. 72:440–447 (1982).
• Tulving, E. “Memory and consciousness,” Canadian Psychology 26:1–
26 (1985).
• Tulving, E. “Varieties of consciousness and levels of awareness in memory.”
In: Attention: Selection, Awareness and Control. A Tribute to
Donald Broadbent. Baddeley, A. and Weiskrantz, L., eds., pp. 283–
299. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (1993).
• Turing, A. “Computing machinery and intelligence,” Mind 59:433–
460 (1950).
• Ullman, S. “Visual routines,” Cognition 18:97–159 (1984).
• Ungerleider, L.G. and Mishkin, M. “Two cortical visual systems.”
In: Analysis of Visual Behavior. Ingle, D.J., Goodale, M.A., and
Mansfield, R.J.W., eds., pp. 549–586. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1982).
• Vallar, G. and Shallice, T., eds. Neuropsychological Impairments of
Short-Term Memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
(1990).
• Vanduffel, W., Fize, D., Peuskens, H., Denys, K., Sunaert, S., Todd,
J.T., and Orban, G.A. “Extracting 3D from motion: Differences in human
and monkey intraparietal cortex,” Science 298:413–415 (2002).
• Van Essen, D.C. and Gallant, J.L. “Neural mechanisms of form and
motion processing in the primate visual system,” Neuron 13:1–10
(1994).
参考文献84
• Van Essen, D.C., Lewis, J.W., Drury, H.A., Hadjikhani, N., Tootell,
R.B., Bakircioglu, M., and Miller, M.I. “Mapping visual cortex in
monkeys and humans using surface-based atlases,” Vision Res. 41:1359–
1378 (2001).
• VanRullen, R. and Koch, C. “Competition and selection during visual
processing of natural scenes and objects,” J. Vision 3:75–85 (2003a).
• VanRullen, R. and Koch, C. “Visual selective behavior can be triggered
by a feed-forward process,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15:209–217
(2003b).
• VanRullen, R. and Koch, C. “Is perception discrete or continuous?”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:207–213 (2003c).
• VanRullen, R., Reddy L., and Koch, C. “Parallel and preattentive
processing are not equivalent,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., in press (2004).
• VanRullen, R. and Thorpe, S. “The time course of visual processing:
From early perception to decision making,” J. Cogn. Neurosci.
13:454–461 (2001).
• van Swinderen, B. and Greenspan, R.J. “Salience modulates 20–30 Hz
brain activity in Drosophila,” Nature Neurosci. 6:579–586 (2003).
• Varela, F. “Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy to the
hard problem,” J. Consc. Studies 3:330–350 (1996).
• Varela, F., Lachaux, J.-P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. “The
brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration,” Nature
Rev. Neurosci. 2:229–239 (2001).
• Velmans, M. “Is human information processing conscious?” Behav.
Brain Sci. 14:651–726 (1991).
• Venables, P.H. “Periodicity in reaction time,” Br. J. Psychol. 51:37–
43 (1960).
• Vgontzas, A.N. and Kales, A. “Sleep and its disorders,” Ann. Rev.
Med. 50:387–400 (1999).
• Vogeley, K. “Hallucinations emerge from an imbalance of self-monitoring
and reality modeling,” Monist 82:626–644 (1999).
• Volkmann, F.C., Riggs, L.A., and Moore, R.K. “Eyeblinks and visual
suppression,” Science 207:900–902 (1980).
参考文献85
• von der Heydt, R., Peterhans, E., and Baumgartner, G. “Illusory contours
and cortical neuron responses,” Science 224:1260–1262 (1984).
• von der Heydt, R., Zhou, H., and Friedman, H.S. “Representation of
stereoscopic edges in monkey visual cortex,” Vision Res. 40:1955–
1967 (2000).
• von der Malsburg, C. “The correlation theory of brain function.” MPI
Biophysical Chemistry, Internal Report 81–2 (1981). Reprinted in
Models of Neural Networks II, Domany, E., van Hemmen, J.L., and
Schulten, K., eds. Berlin: Springer (1994).
• von der Malsburg, C. “Binding in models of perception and brain
function,” Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5:520–526 (1995).
• von der Malsburg, C. “The what and why of binding: The modeler’s
perspective,” Neuron 24:95–104 (1999).
• von Economo, C. and Koskinas, G.N. Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde
des erwachsenen Menschen. Wien, Austria: Julius Springer
(1925).
• von Helmholtz, H. Handbook of Physiological Optics. New York: Dover.
(1962). Translation of Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. 3 volumes,
ed. and trans. by Southall, J.P.C., Hamburg, Voss, 1856, 1860,
and 1988.
• Von Senden, M. Space and Sight: The Perception of Space and Shape
in the Congenitally Blind Before and After Operation. Glencoe, IL:
Free Press (1960).
• Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J.L., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Driver, J.,
and Dolan, R.J. “Neural response to emotional faces with and without
awareness: Event-related fMRI in a parietal patient with visual
extinction and spatial neglect,” Neuropsychologia 40:156–166 (2002).
• Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J.L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R.J. “Effects
of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An
event-related fMRI study,” Neuron 30:829–841 (2001).
• Vuilleumier, P., Hester, D., Assal, G., and Regli, F. “Unilateral spatial
neglect recovery after sequential strokes,” Neurol. 46:184–189 (1996).
• Wachtler, T., Sejnowski, T.J., and Albright, T.D. “Representation
of color stimuli in awake macaque primary visual cortex,” Neuron
37:681–691 (2003).
参考文献86
• Wada, Y. and Yamamoto, T. “Selective impairment of facial recognition
due to a haematoma restricted to the right fusiform and lateral
occipital region,” J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 71:254–257
(2001).
• Wade, A.R., Brewer, A.A., Rieger, J.W., and Wandell, B.A. “Functional
measurements of human ventral occipital cortex: Retinotopy
and colour,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357:963–973 (2002).
• Walther, D., Itti, L., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Koch, C. “Attentional
selection for object recognition—A gentle way.” In: Biologically
Motivated Computer Vision. B¨ulthoff, H.H., Lee, S.-W., Poggio,
T., and Wallraven, C., eds., pp. 472–479. Berlin: Springer (2002).
• Wandell, B.A. Foundations of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer (1995).
• Wang, G., Tanaka, K., and Tanifuji, M. “Optical imaging of functional
organization in the monkey inferotemporal cortex,” Science
272:1665–1668 (1996).
• Warland, D.K., Reinagel, P., and Meister, M. “Decoding visual information
from a population of retinal ganglion cells,” J. Neurophysiol.
78:2336–2350 (1997).
• Watanabe, T., Harner, A.M., Miyauchi, S., Sasaki, Y., Nielsen, M.,
Palomo, D., and Mukai, I. “Task-dependent influences of attention on
the activation of human primary visual cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. USA 95:11489–11492 (1998).
• Watanabe, M. and Rodieck, R.W. “Parasol and midget ganglion cells
of the primate retina,” J. Comp. Neurol. 289:434–454 (1989).
• Watkins, J.C. and Collingridge, G.L., eds. The NMDA Receptor. Oxford,
UK: IRL Press (1989).
• Watson, L. Jacobson’s Organ and the Remarkable Nature of Smell.
New York: Plume Books (2001).
• Webster, M.J., Bachevalier, J., and Ungerleider, L.G. “Connections of
inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex
in macaque monkeys,” Cerebral Cortex 4:470–483 (1994).
• Wegner, D.M. The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (2002).
参考文献87
• Weiskrantz, L. “Blindsight revisited,” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 6:215–
220 (1996).
• Weiskrantz, L. Consciousness Lost and Found. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press (1997).
• Weller, L.,Weller, A., Koresh-Kamin, H., and Ben-Shoshan, R.“Menstrual
synchrony in a sample of working women,” Psychoneuroendocrinology
24:449–459 (1999).
• Wen, J., Koch, C., and Braun, J. “Spatial vision thresholds in the
near absence of attention,” Vision Res. 37:2409–2418 (1997).
• Wertheimer, M. “Experimentelle Studien ¨uber das Sehen von Bewegung,”
Z. Psychologie 61:161–265 (1912).
• Wessinger, C.M., Fendrich, R., and Gazzaniga, M.S. “Islands of residual
vision in hemianopic patients,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:203–211
(1997).
• Westheimer, G. and McKee, S.P. “Perception of temporal order in
adjacent visual stimuli,” Vision Res. 17:887–892 (1977).
• Whinnery, J.E. and Whinnery, A.M. “Acceleration-induced loss of
consciousness,” Archive Neurol. 47:764–776 (1990).
• White, C. “Temporal numerosity and the psychological unit of duration,”
Psychol. Monographs: General & Appl. 77:1–37 (1963).
• White, C. and Harter, M.R. “Intermittency in reaction time and perception,
and evoked response correlates of image quality,” Acta Psychol.
30:368–377 (1969).
• White, E.L. Cortical Circuits. Boston: Birkh¨auser (1989).
• Wigan, A.L. “Duality of the mind, proved by the structure, functions,
and diseases of the brain,” Lancet 1 :39–41 (1844).
• Wilken, P.C. “Capacity limits for the detection and identification of
change: Implications for models of visual short-term memory.” Ph.D.
Thesis. University of Melbourne, Australia (2001).
• Wilkins, A.J., Shallice, T., and McCarthy, R. “Frontal lesions and
sustained attention,” Neuropsychologia 25:359–65 (1987).
• Williams, D.R., MacLeod, D.E.A., and Hayhoe, M.M. “Foveal tritanopia,”
Vision Res. 21:1341–1356 (1981).
参考文献88
• Williams, D.R., Sekiguchi, N., Haake, W., Brainard, D., and Packer,
O. “The cost of trichromacy for spatial vision.” In: Pigments to
Perception. Lee, B. and Valberg, A., eds., pp. 11–22. New York:
Plenum Press (1991).
• Williams, S.R. and Stuart, G.J. “Dependence of EPSP efficacy on
synapse location in neocortical pyramidal neurons,” Science 295:1907–
1910 (2002).
• Williams, S.R. and Stuart, G.J. “Role of dendritic synapse location in
the control of action potential output,” Trends Neurosci. 26:147–154
(2003).
• Williams, T. The Milk Train Doesn’t Stop Here Anymore. Norfolk,
CT: A New Directions Book (1964).
• Williams, Z.M., Elfar, J.C., Eskandar, E.N., Toth, L.J., and Assad,
J.A. “Parietal activity and the perceived direction of ambiguous apparent
motion,” Nature Neurosci. 6:616–623 (2003).
• Wilson, B.A. and Wearing, D. “Prisoner of consciousness: A state
of just awakening following Herpes Simplex Encephalitis.” In: Broken
Memories: Neuropsychological Case Studies. Campbell, R. and
Conway, M., eds., pp. 15–30. Oxford, UK: Blackwell (1995).
• Wilson, H.R., Levi, D., Maffei, L., Rovamo, J., and DeValois, R. “The
Perception of Form: Retina to Striate Cortex.” In: Visual Perception:
The Neurophysiological Foundations. Spillman, L. and Werner, J.S.,
eds., pp. 231–272. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (1990).
• Wilson, M.A. and McNaughton, B.L. “Dynamics of the hippocampal
ensemble code for space,” Science 261:1055–1058 (1993).
• Wittenberg, G.M. and Tsien, J.Z. “An emerging molecular and cellular
framework for memory processing by the hippocampus,” Trends
Neurosci. 25:501–505 (2002).
• Wojciulik, E. and Kanwisher, N. “Implicit but not explicit feature
binding in a Balint’s patient,” Visual Cognition 5:157–181 (1998).
• Wolfe, J.M. “Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single
flash,” Vision Res. 24:471–478 (1984).
• Wolfe, J.M. “‘Effortless’ texture segmentation and ‘parallel’ visual
search are not the same thing,” Vision Res. 32:757–763 (1992).
参考文献89
• Wolfe, J.M. “Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search,”
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1:202–238 (1994).
• Wolfe, J.M. “Visual Search.” In: The Psychology of Attention. Pashler,
H., ed., pp. 13–73. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1998a).
• Wolfe, J.M. “Visual Memory: What do you know about what you
saw?” Curr. Biol. 8:R303–R304 (1998b).
• Wolfe, J.M. “Inattentional amnesia.” In: Fleeting Memories. Coltheart,
V., ed., pp. 71–94. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1999).
• Wolfe, J.M. and Bennett, S.C. “Preattentive object files: Shapeless
bundles of basic features,” Vision Res. 37:25–44 (1997).
• Wolfe, J.M. and Cave, K.R. “The psychophysical evidence for a binding
problem in human vision,” Neuron 24:11–17 (1999).
• Wong, E. and Mack, A. “Saccadic programming and perceived location,”
Acta Psychologica 48:123–131 (1981).
• Wong-Riley, M.T.T. “Primate visual cortex: Dynamic metabolic organization
and plasticity revealed by cytochrome oxidase.” In: Cerebral
Cortex. Vol. 10. Peters, A. and Rockland, K.S., eds., pp. 141–200.
New York: Plenum Press (1994).
• Woolf, N.J. “Cholinergic transmission: Novel signal transduction.”
In: Neurochemistry of Consciousness. Perry, E., Ashton, H., and
Young, A., eds., pp. 25–41. Amsterdam: John Benjamins (2002).
• Wu, M.-F., Gulyani, S.A., Yau, E., Mignot, E., Phan, B., and Siegel,
J.M. “Locus coeruleus neurons: Cessation of activity during cataplexy,”
Neurosci. 91:1389–1399 (1999).
• Wurtz, R.H., Goldberg, M.E., and Robinson, D.L. “Brain mechanisms
of visual attention,” Sci. Am. 246:124–135 (1982).
• Yabuta, N.H., Sawatari, A., and Callaway, E.M. “Two functional
channels from primary visual cortex to dorsal visual cortical areas,”
Science 292:297–300 (2001).
• Yamagishi, N., Anderson, S.J., and Ashida H. “Evidence for dissociation
between the perceptual and visuomotor systems in humans,”
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268:973–977 (2001).
参考文献90
• Yamamoto, M., Wada, N., Kitabatake, Y., Watanabe, D., Anzai, M.,
Yokoyama, M., Teranishi, Y., and Nakanishi, S. “Reversible suppression
of glutamatergic neurotransmission of cerebellar granule cells in
vivo by genetically manipulated expression of tetanus neurotoxin light
chain,” J. Neurosci. 23:6759–6767 (2003).
• Yang, Y., Rose, D., and Blake, R. “On the variety of percepts associated
with dichoptic viewing of dissimilar monocular stimuli,” Perception
21:47–62 (1992).
• Young, M.P. “Connectional organisation and function in the macaque
cerebral cortex. In: Cortical Areas: Unity and Diversity, Sch¨uz, A.
and Miller, R., eds., pp. 351–375. London: Taylor and Francis (2002).
• Young, M.P. and Yamane, S. “Sparse population coding of faces in
the inferotemporal cortex,” Science 256:1327–1331 (1992).
• Yund, E.W., Morgan, H., and Efron, R. “The micropattern effect and
visible persistence,” Perception & Psychophysics 34:209–213 (1983).
• Zafonte, R.D. and Zasler, N.D. “The minimally conscious state: Definition
and diagnostic criteria,” Neurology 58:349–353 (2002).
• Zeki, S. “Color coding in rhesus monkey prestriate cortex,” Brain Res.
27:422–427 (1973).
• Zeki, S. “Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank
of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey,” J. Physiol.
236:549–573 (1974).
• Zeki, S.“Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelengthselective
and color-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in
wavelength composition,” Neurosci. 9:767–781 (1983).
• Zeki, S. “A century of cerebral achromatopsia,” Brain 113:1721–1777
(1990).
• Zeki, S. “Cerebral akinetopsia (Visual motion blindness),” Brain 114:811–
824 (1991).
• Zeki, S. A Vision of the Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
(1993).
• Zeki, S. “The motion vision of the blind,” Neuroimage 2:231–235
(1995).
参考文献91
• Zeki, S. “Parallel processing, asynchronous perception, and a distributed
system of consciousness in vision,” Neuroscientist 4:365–372
(1998).
• Zeki, S. “Localization and globalization in conscious vision,” Ann.
Rev. Neurosci. 24:57–86 (2001).
• Zeki, S. “Improbable areas in the visual brain,” Trends Neurosci.
26:23–26 (2003).
• Zeki, S. and Bartels, A. “Toward a theory of visual consciousness,”
Consc. & Cognition 8:225–259 (1999).
• Zeki, S., McKeefry, D.J., Bartels, A., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. “Has a
new color area been discovered?” Nature Neurosci. 1:335–336 (1998).
• Zeki, S. and Moutoussis, K. “Temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive
systems in the Mondrian world,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
264:1415–1419 (1997).
• Zeki, S. and Shipp, S. “The functional logic of cortical connections,”
Nature 335:311–317 (1988).
• Zeki, S., Watson, J.D., Lueck, C.J., Friston, K.J., Kennard, C., and
Frackowiak, R.S.J. “A direct demonstration of functional specialization
in human visual cortex,” J. Neurosci. 11:641–649 (1991).
• Zeki, S., Watson, J.D., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. “Going beyond the
information given: The relation of illusory motion to brain activity,”
Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 252:215–222 (1993).
• Zeman, A. “Consciousness,” Brain 124:1263–1289 (2001).
• Zhang, K., Ginzburg, I., McNaughton, B.L., and Sejnowski, T.J. “Interpreting
neuronal population activity by reconstruction: Unified
framework with application to hippocampal place cells,” J. Neurophysiol.
79:1017–1044 (1998).
• Zihl J., von Cramon, D., and Mai, N. “Selective disturbance of movement
vision after bilateral brain-damage,” Brain 106:313–340 (1983).
• Zipser, D. and Andersen, R.A. “A back-propagation programmed network
that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal
neurons,” Nature 331:679–684 (1988).
参考文献92
• Zrenner, E. Neurophysiological Aspects of Color Vision in Primates:
Comparative Studies on Simian Retinal Ganglion Cells and the Human
Visual System. Berlin: Springer (1983).



共通テーマ:健康

『意識の探求』の参考文献60-79

参考文献60
• Rockland, K.S., Kaas, J.H., and Peters, A., eds., pp. 205–241. New
York: Plenum (1997).
• Nunn, J.A., Gregory, L.J., Brammer, M., Williams, S.C.R., Parslow,
D.M., Morgan, M.J., Morris, R.G., Bullmore, E.T., Baron-Cohen, S.,
and Gray, J.A. “Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia:
Activation of V4/V8 by spoken words,” Nature Neurosci. 5:371–
375 (2002).
• O’Connor, D.H., Fukui, M.M., Pinsk, M.A., and Kastner, S. “Attention
modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus,”
Nature Neurosci. 5:1203–1209 (2002).
• O’Craven, K. and Kanwisher, N. “Mental imagery of faces and places
activates corresponding stimulus-specific brain regions,” J. Cogn. Neursci.
12:1013–1023 (2000).
• ¨Ohman, A. and Soares, J.J. “Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli:
Expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fearrelevant
stimuli,” J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 127:69–82 (1998).
• Ojemann, G.A., Ojemann, S.G., and Fried, I. “Lessons from the human
brain: Neuronal activity related to cognition,” Neuroscientist
4:285–300 (1998).
• Ojima, H. “Terminal morphology and distribution of corticothalamic
fibers originating from layers 5 and 6 of cat primary auditory cortex,”
Cerebral Cortex 4:646–663 (1994).
• O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford,
UK: Clarendon (1978).
• O’Keefe, J. and Recce, M.L. “Phase relationship bteween hippocampal
place units and the EEG theta rhythm,” Hippocampus 3:317–330
(1993).
• Ono, H. and Barbeito, R. “Ultocular discrimination is not sufficient
for utrocular identification,” Vision Res. 25:289–299 (1985).
• O’Regan, J.K. “Solving the ‘real’ mysteries of visual perception: The
world as an outside memory,” Canadian J. Psychol. 46:461–488
(1992).
• O’Regan, J.K. and No¨e, A. “A sensorimotor account of vision and
visual consciousness,” Behav. Brain Sci. 24:939–1001 (2001).
参考文献61
• O’Regan, J.K., Rensink, R.A., and Clark, J.J. “Change-blindness as
a result of mudsplashes,” Nature 398:34 (1999).
• O’Shea, R.P. and Corballis, P.M. “Binocular rivalry between complex
stimuli in split-brain observers,” Brain & Mind 2:151–160 (2001).
• Oxbury, J., Polkey, C.E., and Duchowny, M., eds. Intractable Focal
Epilepsy. Philadelphia: Saunders (2000).
• Pagels, H. The Dreams of Reason. New York: Simon and Schuster
(1988).
• Palm, G. Neural Assemblies: An Alternative Approach to Artificial
Intelligence. Berlin: Springer (1982).
• Palm, G. “Cell assemblies as a guideline for brain research,” Concepts
Neurosci. 1:133–147 (1990).
• Palmer, L.A., Jones, J.P., and Stepnoski, R.A. “Striate receptive fields
as linear filters: Characterization in two dimensions of space.” In: The
Neural Basis of Visual Function. Leventhal, A.G., ed., pp. 246–265.
Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (1991).
• Palmer, S. Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1999).
• Pantages, E. and Dulac, C. “A novel family of candidate pheromone
receptors in mammals,” Neuron 28:835–845 (2000).
• Parasuraman, R., ed. The Attentive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1998).
• Parker, A.J. and Krug, K. “Neuronal mechanisms for the perception
of ambiguous stimuli,” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 13:433–439 (2003).
• Parker, A.J. and Newsome, W.T. “Sense and the single neuron: Probing
the physiology of perception,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 21:227–277
(1998).
• Parra, G., Gulyas, A.I., and Miles, R. “How many subtypes of inhibitory
cells in the hippocampus?” Neuron 20:983–993 (1998).
• Parvizi, J. and Damasio, A.R. “Consciousness and the brainstem,”
Cognition 79:135–159 (2001).
• Pashler, H.E. The Psychology of Attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1998).
参考文献62
• Passingham, R. The Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press (1993).
• Pastor, M.A. and Artieda, J., eds. Time, Internal Clocks, and Movement.
Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier (1996).
• Paulesu, E., Harrison, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Watson, J.D., Goldstein,
L., Heather, J., Frackowiak, R.S.J., and Frith, C.D. “The physiology
of coloured hearing. A PET activation study of colour-word synaesthesia,”
Brain 118:661–676 (1995).
• Payne, B.R., Lomber, S.G., Villa, A.E., and Bullier, J. “Reversible deactivation
of cerebral network components,” Trends Neurosci. 19:535–
542 (1996).
• Pedley, T.A. and Guilleminault, C. “Episodic nocturnal wanderings
responsive to anticonvulsant drug therapy,” Ann. Neurol. 2:30–35
(1977).
• Penfield, W. The Mystery of the Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press (1975).(邦訳)塚田裕三, 山河宏訳,脳と心の正体,
法政大学出版局(1987).
• Penfield, W. and Jasper, H. Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of
the Human Brain. Boston: Little & Brown (1954).
• Penfield, W. and Perot, P. “The brain’s record of auditory and visual
experience: A final summary and discussion,” Brain 86:595–696
(1963).
• Penrose, R. The Emperor’s New Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press (1989).(邦訳)林一訳,皇帝の新しい心: コンピュータ・心・
物理法則,みすず書房(1994).
• Penrose, R. Shadows of the Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press (1994).(邦訳)林一訳,心の影: 意識をめぐる未知の科学を探
る1, 2,みすず書房(2001, 2002).
• Perenin, M.T. and Rossetti, Y. “Grasping without form discrimination
in a hemianopic field,” Neuroreport 7:793–797 (1996).
• Perez-Orive, J., Mazor, O., Turner, G.C., Cassenaer, S., Wilson, R.I.,
and Laurent, G. “Oscillations and sparsening of odor representation
in the mushroom body,” Science 297:359–365 (2002).
参考文献63
• Perrett, D.I., Hietanen, J.K., Oram, M.W., and Benson, P.J. “Organization
and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal
cortex,” Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 335:23–30 (1992).
• Perry, E., Ashton, H., and Young, A., eds. Neurochemistry of Consciousness.
Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins (2002).
• Perry, E., Walker, M., Grace, J., and Perry, R. “Acetylcholine in
mind: A neurotransmitter correlate of consciousness,” Trends Neurosci.
22:273–280 (1999).
• Perry, E. and Young, A. “Neurotransmitter networks.” In: Neurochemistry
of Consciousness. Perry, E., Ashton, H., and Young, A.,
eds., pp. 3–23. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins (2002).
• Pessoa, L. and DeWeerd, P., eds. Filling-In: From Perceptual Completion
to Cortical Reorganization. New York: Oxford University Press
(2003).
• Pessoa, L., Thompson, E., and No¨e, A. “Finding out about filling in:
A guide to perceptual completion for visual science and the philosophy
of perception,” Behavioral and Brain Sci. 21:723–802 (1998).
• Peterhans, E. “Functional organization of area V2 in the awake monkey.”
In: Cerebral Cortex, Vol 12. Rockland, K.S., Kaas, J.H., and
Peters, A., eds., pp. 335–358. New York: Plenum Press (1997).
• Peterhans, E. and von der Heydt, R. “Subjective contours: Bridging
the gap between psychophysics and physiology,” Trends Neurosci.
14:112–119 (1991).
• Peters, A. and Rockland, K.S., eds. Cerebral Cortex. Vol. 10. New
York: Plenum Press (1994).
• Pettigrew, J.D. and Miller, S.M. “A ‘sticky’ interhemishperic switch in
bipolar disorder?” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 265:2141–2148
(1998).
• Philbeck, J.W. and Loomis, J.M. “Comparisons of two indicators of
perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions,”
J. Exp. Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 23:72–
85 (1997).
• Pickersgill, M.J. “On knowing with which eye one is seeing,” Quart.
J. Exp. Psychol. 13:168–172 (1961).
参考文献64
• Pitts, W. and McCulloch, W.S. “How we know universals: The perception
of auditory and visual forms,” Bull. Math. Biophysics 9:127–
147 (1947).
• Plum, F. and Posner, J.B. The Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma. 3rd
ed. Philadelphia: FA Davis (1983).
• Pochon, J.-B., Levy, R., Poline, J.-B., Crozier, S., Leh´ericy, S., Pillon,
B., Deweer, B., Le Bihan, D., and Dubois, B. “The role of dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex in the preparation of forthcoming actions: An fMRI
study,” Cerebral Cortex 11:260–266 (2001).
• Poggio, G.F. and Poggio, T. “The analysis of stereopsis,” Ann. Rev.
Neurosci. 7:379–412 (1984).
• Poggio, T. “A theory of how the brain might work,” Cold Spring
Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 55:899–910 (1990).
• Poggio, T., Torre, V., and Koch, C. “Computational vision and regularization
theory,” Nature 317:314–319 (1985).
• Poincar´e, H. “Mathematical discovery.” In: Science and Method. pp.
46–63. New York: Dover Books (1952).(邦訳)吉田洋一訳,科学と
方法,岩波文庫(1953).
• Pollen, D.A. “Cortical areas in visual awareness,” Nature 377:293–294
(1995).
• Pollen, D.A. “On the neural correlates of visual perception,” Cerebral
Cortex 9:4–19 (1999).
• Pollen, D.A. “Explicit neural representations, recursive neural networks
and conscious visual perception,” Cerebral Cortex 13:807–814
(2003).
• Polonsky, A., Blake, R., Braun, J., and Heeger, D. “Neuronal activity
in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during
binocular rivalry,” Nature Neurosci. 3:1153–1159 (2000).
• Polyak, S.L. The Retina. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
(1941).
• P¨oppel, E. “Time perception.” In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology.
Vol. 8: Perception. Held, R., Leibowitz, H.W., and Teuber, H.-L.
eds., pp. 713–729. Berlin: Springer (1978).
参考文献65
• P¨oppel, E. “A hierarchical model of temporal perception,” Trends
Cogn. Sci. 1:56–61 (1997).
• P¨oppel, E., Held, R., and Frost, D. “Residual visual function after
brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man,” Nature
243:295–296 (1973).
• P¨oppel, E. and Logothetis, N.K. “Neural oscillations in the brain.
Discontinuous initiations of pursuit eye movements indicate a 30-
Hz temporal framework for visual information processing,” Naturwissenschaften
73:267–268 (1986).
• Popper, K.R. and Eccles, J.C. The Self and its Brain. Berlin: Springer
(1977).(邦訳)大村裕, 西脇与作, 沢田允茂訳,自我と脳,新思索社
(2005).
• Porac, C. and Coren, S. “Sighting dominance and utrocular discrimination,”
Percept. Psychophys. 39:449–41 (1986).
• Posner, M.I. and Gilbert, C.D. “Attention and primary visual cortex,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 16:2585–2587 (1999).
• Posner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R. and Davidson, B.J. “Attention and the
detection of signals,” J. exp. Psychol.: General 109:160–174 (1980).
• Potter, M.C. “Very short-term conceptual memory,” Memory & Cognition
21:156–161 (1993).
• Potter, M.C. and Levy, E.I. “Recognition memory for a rapid sequence
of pictures,” J. Exp. Psychol. 81:10–15 (1969).
• Pouget, A. and Sejnowski, T.J. “Spatial transformations in the parietal
cortex using basis functions,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:222–237
(1997).
• Preuss, T.M. “What’s human about the human brain?” In: The New
Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed., pp. 1219–
1234. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Preuss, T.M., Qi, H., and Kaas, J.H. “Distinctive compartmental organization
of human primary visual cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 96:11601–11606 (1999).
• Pritchard, R.M., Heron, W., and Hebb, D.O. “Visual perception approached
by the method of stabilized images,” Canad. J. Psychol.
14:67–77 (1960).
参考文献66
• Proffitt, D.R., Bhalla, M., Gossweiler, R., and Midgett, J. “Perceiving
geographical slant,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Rev. 2:409–428 (1995).
• Przybyszewski, A.W., Gaska, J.P., Foote, W., and Pollen, D.A. “Striate
cortex increases contrast gain of macaque LGN neurons,” Visual
Neurosci. 17:485–494 (2000).
• Puccetti, R. The Trial of John and Henry Norton. London: Hutchinson
(1973).
• Purpura, K.P. and Schiff, N.D. “The thalamic intralaminar nuclei:
Role in visual awareness,” Neuroscientist 3:8–14 (1997).
• Purves, D., Paydarfar, J.A., and Andrews, T.J. “The wagon wheel
illusion in movies and reality,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93:3693–
3697 (1996).
• Quinn, J.J., Oommen, S.S., Morrison, G.E., and Fanselow,M.S. “Posttraining
excitotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus attenuate forward
trace, backward trace, and delay fear conditioning in a temporallyspecific
manner,” Hippocampus 12:495–504 (2002).
• Rafal, R.D. “Hemispatial neglect: Cognitive neuropsychological aspects.”
In: Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. Feinberg,
T.E. and Farah, M.J., eds., pp. 319–336. New York: McGraw-Hill
(1997a).
• Rafal, R.D. “Balint syndrome.” In: Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology.
Feinberg, T.E. and Farah, M.J., eds., pp. 337–356. New
York: McGraw-Hill (1997b).
• Rafal, R.D. and Posner, M. “Deficits in human visual spatial attention
following thalamic lesions,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 84:7349–
7353 (1987).
• Rakic, P. “A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: A hypothesis
of neocortical expansion during evolution,” Trends Neurosci.
18:383–388 (1995).
• Ramachandran, V.S. “Blind spots,” Sci. Am. 266:86–91 (1992).
• Ramachandran, V.S. and Gregory, R.L. “Perceptual filling in of artificially
induced scotomas in human vision,” Nature 350:699–702 (1991).
参考文献67
• Ramachandram, V.S. and Hubbard, E.M. “Psychophysical investigations
into the neural basis of synaesthesia,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
268:979–983 (2001).
• Ram`on y Cajal, S. “New ideas on the structure of the nervous system
of man and vertebrates.” Translated by Swanson, N. and Swanson,
L.M. fromLes nouvelles id´ees sur la structure du syst`eme nerveux chez
l’homme et chez les vert´ebr´es. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1991).
• Rao, R.P.N. and Ballard, D.H. “Predictive coding in the visual cortex:
A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field
effects,” Nature Neurosci. 2:79–87 (1999).
• Rao, R.P.N, Olshausen, B.A., and Lewicki, M.S., eds. Probabilistic
Models of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2002).
• Rao, S.C., Rainer, G., and Miller, E.K. “Integration of what and
where in the primate prefrontal cortex,” Science 276:821–824 (1997).
• Ratliff, F. and Hartline, H.K. “The responses of Limulus optic nerve
fibers to patterns of illumination on the receptor mosaic,” J. Gen.
Physiol. 42:1241–1255 (1959).
• Ray, P.G., Meador, K.J., Smith, J.R., Wheless, J.W., Sittenfeld, M.,
and Clifton, G.L. “Cortical stimulation and recording in humans,”
Neurology 52:1044–1049 (1999).
• Reddy, L., Wilken, P., and Koch, C. “Face-gender discrimination in
the near-absence of attention,” J. Vision, in press (2004).
• Rees, G., Friston, K., and Koch, C. “A direct quantitative relationship
between the functional properties of human and macaque V5,” Nature
Neurosci. 3:716–723 (2000).
• Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., and Driver,
J. “Unconscious activation of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere
of a parietal patient with extinction,” Brain 123:1624–1633
(2000).
• Reeves, A.G., ed. Epilepsy and the Corpus Callosum. New York:
Plenum Press (1985).
• Reingold, E.M. and Merikle, P.M. “On the inter-relatedness of theory
and measurement in the study of unconscious processes,” Mind Lang.
5:9–28 (1990).
参考文献68
• Rempel-Clower, N.L. and Barbas, H. “The laminar pattern of connections
between prefrontal and anterior temporal cortices in the rhesus
monkey is related to cortical structure and function,” Cerebral Cortex
10:851–865 (2000).
• Rensink, R.A. “Seeing, sensing, and scrutinizing,” Vision Res. 40:1469–
1487 (2000a).
• Rensink, R.A. “The dynamic representation of scenes,” Visual Cognition
7:17–42 (2000b).
• Rensink, R.A., O’Regan, J.K., and Clark, J.J. “To see or not to see:
The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes,” Psychological
Sci. 8:368–373 (1997).
• Revonsuo, A. “The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis
of the function of dreaming,” Behav. Brain Sci. 23:877–901
(2000).
• Revonsuo, A., Johanson, M., Wedlund, J.-E., and Chaplin, J. “The
zombie among us.” In: Beyond Dissociation. Rossetti, Y. and Revonsuo,
A., eds., pp. 331–351. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins
(2000).
• Revonsuo, A., Wilenius-Emet, M., Kuusela, J., and Lehto, M. “The
neural generation of a unified illusion in human vision,” Neuroreport
8:3867–3870 (1997).
• Reynolds, J.H., Chelazzi, L., and Desimone, R. “Competitive mechanisms
subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4,” J. Neurosci.
19:1736–1753 (1999).
• Reynolds, J.H. and Desimone, R. “The role of neural mechanisms of
attention in solving the binding problem,” Neuron 24:19–29 (1999).
• Rhodes P.A. and Llin´as, R.R. “Apical tuft input efficacy in layer
5 pyramidal cells from rat visual cortex,” J. Physiol. 536:167–187
(2001).
• Ricci, C. and Blundo, C. “Perception of ambiguous figures after focal
brain lesions,” Neuropsychologia 28:1163–73 (1990).
• Riddoch, M.J. and Humphreys, G.W. “17 + 14 = 41? Three cases of
working memory impairment.” In: Broken Memories: Case Studies
in Memory Impairment. Campbell, R. and Conway, M.A., eds., pp.
253–266. Oxford, UK: Blackwell (1995).
参考文献69
• Ridley, M. Nature Via Nurture. New York: Harper Collins (2003).
• Rieke, F., Warland, D., van Steveninck, R.R.D., and Bialek, W.
Spikes: Exploring the Neural Code. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1996).
• Ritz, R. and Sejnowski, T.J. “Synchronous oscillatory activity in sensory
systems: New vistas on mechanisms,” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol.
7:536–546 (1997).
• Rizzuto, D.S., Madsen, J.R., Bromfield, E.B., Schulze-Bonhage, A.,
Seelig, D., Aschen-brenner-Scheibe, R., and Kahana, M.J. “Reset of
human neocortical oscillations during a working memory task,” Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100:7931–7936 (2003).
• Robertson, L. “Binding, spatial attention, and perceptual awareness,”
Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4:93–102 (2003).
• Robertson, I.H. and Marshall, J.C., eds. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical
and Experimental Studies. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum (1993).
• Robertson, L., Treisman, A., Friedman-Hill, S., and Grabowecky,
M. “The interaction of spatial and object pathways: Evidence from
Balint’s syndrome,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:295–317 (1997).
• Robinson, D.L. and Cowie, R.J. “The primate pulvinar: Stuctural,
functional, and behavioral components of visual salience.” In: The
Thalamus. Jones, E.G., Steriade, M., and McCormick, D.A., eds., pp.
53–92. Amsterdam: Elsevier (1997).
• Robinson, D.L. and Petersen, S.E. “The pulvinar and visual salience,”
Trends Neurosci. 15:127–132 (1992).
• Rock, I. and Gutman, D. “The effect of inattention on form perception,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Perception & Performance 7:275–285
(1981).
• Rockel, A.J., Hiorns, R.W., and Powell, T.P.S. “The basic uniformity
in structure of the neocortex,” Brain 103:221–244 (1980).
• Rockland, K.S. “Further evidence for two types of corticopulvinar
neurons,” Neuroreport 5:1865–1868 (1994).
• Rockland, K.S. “Two types of corticopulvinar terminations: Round
(type 2) and elongate (type 1),” J. Comp. Neurol. 368:57–87 (1996).
参考文献70
• Rockland, K.S. “Elements of cortical architecture: Hierarchy revisited.”
In: Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 12. Rockland, K.S., Kaas, J.H., and
Peters, A., eds., pp. 243–293. New York: Plenum Press (1997).
• Rockland, K.S. and Pandya, D.N. “Laminar origins and terminations
of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey,”
Brain Res. 179:3–20 (1979).
• Rockland, K.S. and Van Hoesen, G.W. “Direct temporal-occipital
feedback connections to striate cortex (V1) in the macaque monkey,”
Cerebral Cortex 4:300–313 (1994).
• Rodieck, R.W. The First Steps in Seeing. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer
Associates (1998).
• Rodieck, R.W., Binmoeller, K.F., and Dineen, J.T. “Parasol and
midget ganglion cells of the human retina,” J. Comp. Neurol. 233:115–
132 (1985).
• Rodriguez, E., George, N., Lachaux, J.-P., Martinerie, J., Renault, B.,
and Varela, F.J. “Perception’s shadow: Long-distance synchronziation
of human brain activity,” Nature 397:430–433 (1999).
• Roe, A.W. and Ts’o, D.Y. “The functional architecture of area V2
in the macaque monkey: Physiology, topography, and connectivity.”
In Cerebral Cortex, Vol 12: Extrastriate Cortex in Primates, Rockland,
K.S., Kaas, J.H., and Peters, A., eds., pp. 295–334. New York:
Plenum Press (1997).
• Roelfsema, P.R., Lamme, V.A.F., and Spekreijse, H. “Oject-based
attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey,” Nature
395:376–381 (1998).
• Rolls, E.T. “Spatial view cells and the representation of place in the
primate hippocampus,” Hippocampus 9:467–480 (1999).
• Rolls, E.T., Aggelopoulos, N.C., and Zheng, F. “The receptive fields
of inferior temporal cortex neurons in natural scenes,” J. Neurosci.
23:339–348 (2003).
• Rolls, E.T. and Deco, G. Computational Neuroscience of Vision. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press (2002).
• Rolls, E.T. and Tovee, M.J. “Processing speed in the cerebral cortex
and the neurophysiology of visual masking,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
257:9–15 (1994).
参考文献71
• Rolls, E.T. and Tovee, M.J. “The responses of single neurons in the
temporal visual cortical areas of the macaque when more than one
stimulus is present in the receptive field,” Exp. Brain Res. 103:409–
420 (1995).
• Romo, R., Brody, C.D., Hern´andez, A., and Lemus, L. “Neuronal
correlates of parametric working memory in the prefrontal cortex,”
Nature 399:470–473 (1999).
• Roorda, A. and Williams, D.R. “The arrangement of the three cone
classes in the living human eye,” Nature 397:520–522 (1999).
• Rosen, M. and Lunn, J.N., eds. Consciousness, Awareness, and Pain
in General Anaesthesia. London: Butterworths (1987).
• Rossen, R., Kabat, H., and Anderson, J.P. “Acute arrest of cerebral
circulation in man,” Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 50:510–528 (1943).
• Rossetti, Y. “Implicit short-lived motor representations of space in
brain damaged and healthy subjects,” Consc. & Cognition 7:520–558
(1998).
• Rousselet, G., Fabre-Thorpe, M., and Thorpe, S. “Parallel processing
in high-level visual scene categorization,” Nature Neurosci. 5:629–630
(2002).
• Ryle, G. The Concept of the Mind London: Hutchinson (1949).(邦
訳)坂本百大〔ほか〕共訳,心の概念,みすず書房(1987).
• Sacks, O. Migraine. Rev. ed. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press (1970).(邦訳)後藤真, 石館宇夫訳,偏頭痛百科,晶文社(1990).
• Sacks, O. Awakenings. New York: E.P. Dutton (1973).(邦訳)石館
康平, 石館宇夫訳,レナードの朝,晶文社(1993).
• Sacks, O. A Leg to Stand On. New York: Summit Books (1984).(邦
訳)金沢泰子訳,左足をとりもどすまで,晶文社(1994).
• Sacks, O. The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat. New York:
Harper & Row (1985).(邦訳)高見幸郎, 金沢泰子訳,妻を帽子とまち
がえた男,晶文社(1992).
• Sacks, O. “The mind’s eye: What the blind see.” The New Yorker,
July 28, pp. 48–59 (2003).
参考文献72
• Saenz, M., Buracas, G.T., and Boynton, G.M. “Global effects of
feature-based attention in human visual cortex,” Nature Neurosci.
5:631–632 (2002).
• Saint-Cyr, J.A., Ungerleider, L.G., and Desimone, R. “Organization
of visual cortical inputs to the striatum and subsequent outputs to the
pallido-nigral complex in the monkey,” J. Compa. Neurol. 298:129–
156 (1990).
• Sakai, K., Watanabe, E., Onodera, Y., Uchida, I., Kato, H., Yamamoto,
E., Koizumi, H., and Miyashita, Y. “Functional mapping of
the human colour centre with echo-planar magnetic resonance imaging,”
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 261:89–98 (1995).
• Saleem, K.S., Suzuki, W., Tanaka, K., and Hashikawa, T. “Connections
between anterior inferotemporal cortex and superior temporal
sulcus regions in the macaque monkey,” J. Neurosci. 20:5083–5101
(2000).
• Salin, P.-A. and Bullier, J. “Corticocortical connections in the visual
system: Structure and Function,” Physiol. Rev. 75:107–154 (1995).
• Salinas, E. and Abbott, L.F. “Transfer of coded information from
sensory to motor networks,” J. Neurosci. 15:6461–6474 (1995).
• Salinas, E. and Sejnowski, T.J. “Correlated neuronal activity and the
flow of neural information,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2:539–550 (2001).
• Salzman, C.D., Murasugi, C.M., Britten, K.H., and Newsome, W.T.
“Microstimulation in visual area MT: Effects on direction discrimination
performance,” J. Neurosci. 12:2331–2355 (1992).
• Salzman, C.D. and Newsome, W.T. “Neural mechanisms for forming
a perceptual decision,” Science 264:231–237 (1994).
• Sammon, P.M. Future Noir: The Making of Blade Runner. NewYork,
HarperPrims (1996).
• Sanderson,M.J. “Intercellular waves of communication,” New Physiol.
Sci. 11:262–269 (1996).
• Sanford, A.J. “A periodic basis for perception and action.” In: Biological
Rhythms and Human Performance. Colquhuon, W., ed., pp.
179–209. New York: Academic Press (1971).
参考文献73
• Savic, I. “Imaging of brain activation by odorants in humans,” Curr.
Opinion Neurobiol. 12:455–461 (2002).
• Savic, I., Berglund, H., Gulyas, B., and Roland, P. “Smelling of odorous
sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic
activations in humans,” Neuron 31:661–668 (2001).
• Sawatari, A. and Callaway, E.M. “Diversity and cell type specificity of
local excitatory connections to neurons in layer 3B of monkey primary
visual cortex,” Neuron 25:459–471 (2000).
• Scalaidhe, S.P., Wilson, F.A., and Goldman-Rakic, P.S. “Areal segregation
of face-processing neurons in prefrontal cortex,” Science 278:1135–
1138 (1997).
• Schall, J.D. “Neural basis of saccadic eye movements in primates.”
In: The Neural Basis of Visual Function. Leventhal, A.G., ed., pp.
388–441. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (1991).
• Schall, J.D. “Visuomotor areas of the frontal lobe. In: Cerebral Cortex.
Vol. 12. Rockland, K.S., Kaas, J.H., and Peters, A., eds., pp.
527–638. New York: Plenum Press (1997).
• Schall, J.D. “Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting,” Nature
Rev. Neurosci. 2:33–42 (2001).
• Schank, J.C. “Menstrual-cycle synchrony: Problems and new directions
for research,” J. Comp. Psychology 115:3–15 (2001).
• Schenck, C.H. and Mahowald, M.W. “An analysis of a recent criminal
trial involving sexual misconduct with a child, alcohol abuse and a
successful sleepwalking defence: Arguments supporting two proposed
new forensic categories,” Med. Sci. Law 38:147–152 (1998).
• Schiff, N.D. “The neurology of impaired consciousness: Challenges
for cognitive neuroscience.” In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences.
Gazzaniga, M., ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2004).
• Schiff, N.D. and Plum, F. “The role of arousal and ‘gating’ systems
in the neurology of impaired consciousness,” J. Clinical Neurophysiol.
17:438–452 (2000).
• Schiffer, F. “Can the different cerebral hemispheres have distinct personalities?
Evidence and its implications for theory and treatment of
PTSD and other disorders?” J. Traum. Dissoc. 1:83–104 (2000).
参考文献74
• Schiller, P.H. and Chou, I.H. “The effects of frontal eye field and
dorsomedial frontal-cortex lesions on visually guided eye-movements,”
Nature Neurosci. 1:248–253 (1998).
• Schiller, P.H. and Logothetis, N.K. “The color-opponent and broadbased
channels of the primate visual system,” Trends Neurosci. 13:392–
398 (1990).
• Schiller, P.H., True, S.D., and Conway, J.L. “Effects of frontal eye
field and superior colliculus ablations on eye movements,” Science
206:590–592 (1979).
• Schlag, J. and Schlag-Rey, M. “Visuomotor functions of central thalamus
in monkey. II. Unit activity related to visual events, targeting,
and fixation,” J. Neurophysiol. 51:1175–1195 (1984).
• Schlag, J. and Schlag-Rey, M. “Through the eye, slowly: Delays
and localization errors in the visual system,” Nature Rev. Neurosci.
3:191–215 (2002).
• Schmidt, E.M., Bak, M.J., Hambrecht, F.T., Kufta, C.V., O’Rourke,
D.K., and Vallabhanath, P. “Feasibility of a visual prosthesis for the
blind based on intracortical microstimulation of the visual cortex,”
Brain 119:507–522 (1996).
• Schmolesky, M.T., Wang, Y., Hanes, D.P., Leutgeb, S., Schall, J.B.,
and Leventhal, A.G. “Signal timing across the macaque visual system,”
J. Neurophysiol. 79:3272–3280 (1998).
• Schooler, J.W. and Melcher, J. “The ineffability of insight.” In: The
Creative Cognition Approach. Smith, S.M., Ward, T.B., and Finke,
R.A., eds., pp. 97–133. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1995).
• Schooler, J.W., Ohlsson, S., and Brooks, K. “Thoughts beyond words:
When language overshadows insight,” J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122:166–
183 (1993).
• Schr¨odinger, E.What Is Life? Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press (1944).(邦訳)岡小天, 鎮目恭夫共訳,生命とは何か: 物理学者
のみた生細胞,岩波新書(1951).
• Scoville, W.B. and Milner, B. “Loss of recent memory after bilateral
hippocampal lesions,” J. Neurochem. 20:11–21 (1957).
参考文献75
• Searle, J.R. The Mystery of Consciousness. NewYork: TheNewYork
Review of Books (1997).
• Searle, J.R. “Consciousness,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 23:557–578 (2000).
• Seckel, A. The Art of Optical Illusions. Carlton Books (2000).
• Seckel, A. More Optical Illusions. Carlton Books (2002).
• Sennholz, G. “Bispectral analysis technology and equipment,” Minerva
Anestesiol. 66:386–388 (2000).
• Shadlen, M.N., Britten, K.H., Newsome, W.T., and Movshon, J.A.
“A computational analysis of the relationship between neuronal and
behavioral responses to visual motion,” J. Neurosci. 16:1486–1510
(1996).
• Shadlen, M.N. and Movshon, J.A. “Synchrony unbound: A critical
evaluation of the temporal binding hypothesis,” Neuron 24:67–77
(1999).
• Shallice, T. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press (1988).
• Shapley, R. and Ringach, D. “Dynamics of responses in visual cortex.”
In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed.,
pp. 253–261. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Shear, J., ed. Explaining Consciousness: The Hard Problem. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1997).
• Sheinberg, D.L. and Logothetis, N.K. “The role of temporal cortical
areas in perceptual organization,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
94:3408–3413 (1997).
• Sheinberg, D.L. and Logothetis, N.K. “Noticing familiar objects in
real world scenes: The role of temporal cortical neurons in natural
vision,” J. Neurosci. 15:1340–1350 (2001).
• Sheliga, B.M., Riggio, L., and Rizzolatti, G. “Orienting of attention
and eye movements,” Exp. Brain Res. 98:507–522 (1994).
• Shepherd, G.M. Foundations of the Neuron Doctrine. NewYork: Oxford
University Press (1991).
参考文献76
• Shepherd, M., Findlay, J.M., and Hockey, R.J. “The relationship between
eye movements and spatial attention,” Quart. J. Exp. Psychol.
38:475–491 (1986).
• Sherk, H. “The claustrum.” In: Cerebral Cortex Vol. 5. Jones, E.G.
and Peters, A., eds., pp. 467–499. New York: Plenum (1986).
• Sherman, S.M. and Guillery, R. Exploring the Thalamus. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press (2001).
• Sherman, S.M. and Koch, C. “Thalamus.” In: The Synaptic Organization
of the Brain. 4th ed., Shepherd, G. ed., pp. 289–328. New
York: Oxford University Press (1998).
• Sheth, B.R., Nijhawan, R., and Shimojo, S. “Changing objects lead
briefly flashed ones,” Nature Neurosci. 3:489–495 (2000).
• Shimojo, S., Tanaka, Y., and Watanabe, K. “Stimulus-driven facilitation
and inhibition of visual information processing in environmental
and retinotopic representations of space,” Brain Res. Cogn. Brain
Res. 5:11–21 (1996).
• Siegel, J.M. “Nacrolepsy,” Scientific American 282:76–81 (2000).
• Siewert, C.P. The Significance of Consciousness. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press (1998).
• Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. “Gorillas in our midst: Sustained
inattentional blindness for dynamic events,” Perception 28:1059–1074
(1999).
• Simons, D.J. and Levin, D.T. “Change blindness,” Trends Cogn. Sci.
1:261–267 (1997).
• Simons, D.J. and Levin, D.T. “Failure to detect changes to people
during a real-world interaction,” Psychonomic Bull. & Rev. 5:644–
649 (1998).
• Simpson, J. Touching the Void. New York: HarperPerennial (1988).
• Singer, W. “Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code for the definition
of relations?” Neuron 24:49–65 (1999).
• Skoyles, J.R. “Another variety of vision,” Trends Neurosci. 20:22–23
(1997).
参考文献77
• Slimko, E.M., McKinney, S., Anderson, D.J., Davidson, N., and Lester,
H.A. “Selective electrical silencing of mammalian neurons in vitro by
the use of invertebrate ligand-gated chloride channels,” J. Neurosci.
22:7373–7379 (2002).
• Smith, S. “Utrocular, or ‘which eye’ discrimination,” J. Exp. Psychology
35:1–14 (1945).
• Snyder, L.H., Batista, A.P., and Andersen, R.A. “Intention-related activity
in the posterior parietal cortex: A review,” Vis. Res. 40:1433–
1441 (2000).
• Sobel, E.S. and Tank, D.W. “In vivo Ca2+ dynamics in a cricket auditory
neuron: An example of chemical computation,” Science 263:823–
826 (1994).
• Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Hartely, C.A., Desmond, J.E., Glover,
G.H., Sullivan, E.V., and Gabrieli, D.E. “Blindsmell: Brain activation
induced by an undetected air-borne chemical,” Brain 122:209–217
(1999).
• Softky, W.R. “Simple codes versus efficient codes,” Curr. Opinion
Neurobiol. 5:239–247 (1995).
• Solms, M. The Neuropsychology of Dreams. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum (1997).
• Somers, D.C., Dale, A.M., Seiffert, A.E., and Tootell, R.B. “Functional
MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human
primary visual cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96:1663–1668
(1999).
• Sperling, G. “The information available in brief presentation,” Psychological
Monographs 74. Whole No. 498 (1960).
• Sperling, G. and Dosher, B. “Strategy and optimization in human information
processing.” In: Handbook of Perception and Performance
Vol. 1. Boff, K., Kaufman, L., and Thomas, J., eds., pp. 1–65. New
York: Wiley (1986).
• Sperling, G. andWeichselgartner, E. “Episodic theory of the dynamics
of spatial attention,” Psych. Rev. 102:503–532 (1995).
• Sperry, R.W. “Cerebral organization and behavior,” Science 133:1749–
1757 (1961).
参考文献78
• Sperry, R.W. “Lateral specialization in the surgically separated hemispheres.”
In: Neuroscience 3rd Study Program. Schmitt, F.O. and
Worden, F.G., eds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1974).
• Spinelli, D.W., Pribram, K.H., andWeingarten, M. “Centrifugal optic
nerve responses evoked by auditory and somatic stimulation,” Exp.
Neurol. 12:303–318 (1965).
• Sprague, J.M. “Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation
of visually guided behavior in the cat,” Science 153:1544–1547
(1966).
• Squire, L.R. and Kandel, E.R. Memory: From Mind to Molecules.
New York: Scientific American Library, Freeman (1999).
• Standing, L. “Learning 10,000 pictures,” Quart. J. Exp. Psychol.
25:207–222 (1973).
• Stapledon, O. Star Maker. New York: Dover Publications (1937).
• Steinmetz, P.N., Roy, A., Fitzgerald, P.J., Hsiao, S.S., Johnson, K.O.,
and Niebur, E. “Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in
primary somatosensory cortex,” Nature 404:187–190 (2000).
• Steriade, M. and McCarley, R.W. Brainstem Control of Wakefullness
and Sleep. New York: Plenum Press (1990).
• Stern, K. and McClintock, M.K. “Regulation of ovulation by human
pheromones,” Nature 392:177–179 (1998).
• Sternberg, E.M. “Piercing together a puzzling world: Memento,” Science
292:1661–1662 (2001).
• Sternberg, S. “High-speed scanning in human memory,” Science 153:652–
654 (1966).
• Stevens, C.F. “Neuronal diversity: Too many cell types for comfort?”
Curr. Biol. 8:R708–R710 (1998).
• Stevens, R. “Western phenomenological approaches to the study of
conscious experience and their implications.” In: Methodologies for
the Study of Consciousness: A New Synthesis. Richardson, J. and
Velmans, M., eds., pp. 100–123. Kalamazoo, MI: Fetzer Institute
(1997).
参考文献79
• Stoerig, P. and Barth, E. “Low-level phenomenal vision despite unilateral
destruction of primary visual cortex,” Consc. & Cognition
10:574–587 (2001).
• Stoerig, P., Zontanou, A., and Cowey, A. “Aware or unaware: Assessment
of cortical blindness in four men and a monkey,” Cerebral
Cortex 12:565–574 (2002).
• Stopfer, M., Bhagavan, S., Smith, B.H., and Laurent, G. “Impaired
odour discrimination on desynchronization of odour-encoding neural
assemblies,” Nature 390:70–74 (1997).
• Stowers, L., Holy, T.E., Meister, M., Dulac, C., and Koentges, G.
“Loss of sex discrimination and male-male aggression in mice deficient
for TRP2,” Science 295:1493–1500 (2002).
• Strayer, D.L. and Johnston, W.A. “Driven to distraction: Dual-task
studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular phone,” Psychol.
Sci. 12:462–466 (2001).
• Stroud, J.M. “The fine structure of psychological time.” In: Information
Theory in Psychology. Quastler, H., ed., pp. 174–205. Glencoe,
IL: Free Press (1956).
• Strawson, G. Mental Reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1996).
• Sup`er, H., Spekreijse, H., and Lamme, V.A.F. “Two distinct modes of
sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex,” Nature
Neurosci. 4:304–310 (2001).
• Swick, D. and Knight, R.T. “Cortical lesions and attention.” In: The
Attentive Brain. Parasurama R., ed., pp. 143–161. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press (1998).
• Swindale, N.V. “How many maps are there in visual cortex,” Cerebral
Cortex 10:633–643 (2000).
• Tallal, P., Merzenich, M., Miller, S., and Jenkins, W. “Language learning
impairment: Integrating basic science, technology and remediation,”
Exp. Brain Res. 123:210–219 (1998).
• Tallon-Baudry, C. and Bertrand, O. “Oscillatory gamma activity in
humans and its role in object representation,” Trends Cogn. Sci.
3:151–161 (1999).



共通テーマ:健康

『意識の探求』の参考文献40-59

参考文献40
• Jolicoeur, P., Ullman, S., and MacKay, M. “Curve tracing: A possible
basic operation in the perception of spatial relations,” Mem. Cognition
14:129–140 (1986).
• Jones, E.G. The Thalamus. New York: Plenum Press (1985).
• Jones, E.G. “Thalamic organization and function after Cajal,” Progress
Brain Res. 136:333–357 (2002).
• Jordan, G. and Mollon, J.D. “A study of women heterozygous for
color deficiences,” Vision Res. 33:1495–1508 (1993).
• Jovicich, J., Peters, R.J., Koch, C., Braun, J., Chang, L., and Ernst,
T. “Brain areas specific for attentional load in a motion tracking task,”
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13:1048–1058 (2001).
• Judson, H.J. The Eighth Day of Creation. London: Penguin Books
(1979).
• Julesz, B. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago, IL: University
of Chicago Press (1971).
• Julesz, B. “Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their
interactions,” Nature 290:91–97 (1981).
• Kahana, M.K., Sekuler, R., Caplan, J.B., Kirschen, M., and Madsen,
J.R. “Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual
maze navigation,” Nature 399:781–784 (1999).
• Kamitani, Y. and Shimojo, S. “Manifestation of scotomas created by
transcranial magnetic stimulation of human visual cortex,” Nature
Neurosci. 2:767–771 (1999).
• Kandel, E.R. “A new intellectual framework for psychiatry,” Am. J.
Psychiatry 155:457–469 (1998).
• Kandel, E.R. “The molecular biology of memory storage: A dialogue
between genes and synapses,” Science 294:1030–1038 (2001).
• Kanizsa, G. Organization in Vision: Essays in Gestalt Perception.
New York: Praeger (1979).
• Kanwisher, N. and Driver, J. “Objects, attributes, and visual attention:
Which, what and where,” Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 1:26–31
(1997).
参考文献41
• Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., and Chun, M.M. “The fusiform face
area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception,”
J. Neurosci. 17:4302–4311 (1997).
• Kaplan, E. “The receptive field structure of retinal ganglion cells in cat
and monkey.” In: The Neural Basis of Visual Function. Leventhal,
A.G., ed., pp. 10–40. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (1991).
• Kaplan-Solms, K. and SolmsM. Clinical Studies in Neuro-Psychoanalysis.
London: Karnac Books (2000).
• Karnath, H.-O. “New insights into the functions of the superior temporal
cortex,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2:568–576 (2001).
• Karnath, H.-O., Ferber, S., and Himmelbach, M. “Spatial awareness
is a function of the temporal, not the posterior parietal lobe,” Nature
411:950–954 (2001).
• Kastner, S., DeWeerd, P., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L.G. “Mechanisms
of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed
by functional MRI,” Science 282:108–111 (1998).
• Kastner, S. and Ungerleider, L.G. “Mechanisms of visual attention in
the human cortex,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 23:315–341 (2000).
• Kavey, N.B., Whyte, J., Resor, S.R. Jr., and Gidro-Frank, S. “Somnambulism
in adults,” Neurol. 40:749–752 (1990).
• Keil, A., M¨uller, M.M., Ray, W.J., Gruber, T., and Elbert, T. “Human
gamma band activity and perception of a gestalt,” J. Neurosci.
19:7152–7161 (1999).
• Keller, E.F. The Century of the Gene. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press (2000).
• Kennedy, H. and Bullier, J. “A double-labelling investigation of the afferent
connectivity to cortical areas V1 and V2,” J. Neurosci. 5:2815–
2830 (1985).
• Kentridge, R.W., Heywood, C.A., and Weiskrantz, L. “Residual vision
in multiple retinal locations within a scotoma: Implications for
blindsight,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9:191–202 (1997).
• Kentridge, R.W., Heywood, C.A., andWeiskrantz, L. “Attention without
awareness in blindsight,” Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 266:1805–
1811 (1999).
参考文献42
• Kessel, R.G. and Kardon, R.H. Tissues and Organs: A Text-Atlas of
Scanning Electron Microscopy. San Francisco, CA: Freeman (1979).
• Keverne, E.B. “The vomeronasal organ,” Science 286:716–720 (1999).
• Keysers, C. and Perrett, D.I. “Visual masking and RSVP reveal neural
competition,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 6:120–125 (2002).
• Keysers, C., Xiao, D.-K., F¨oldi´ak, P., and Perrett, D.I. “The speed of
sight,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13:1–12 (2001).
• Kinney, H.C., Korein, J., Panigrahy, A., Dikkes, P., and Goode, R.
“Neuropathological findings in the brain of Karen Ann Quinlan,” New
England J. Med. 330:1469–1475 (1994).
• Kinomura, S., Larsson, J., Guly´as, B., and Roland, P.E. “Activation
by attention of the human reticular formation and thalamic intralaminar
nuclei,” Science 271:512–515 (1996).
• Kirk, R. “Zombies versusmaterialists,” Aristotelian Society 48 (suppl.):
135–152 (1974).
• Kitcher, P. Freud’s Dream: A Complete Interdisciplinary Science of
Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1992).
• Kleinschmidt, A., Buchel, C., Zeki, S., and Frackowiak, R.S.J. “Human
brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous
figures,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265:2427–2433 (1998).
• Klemm, W.R., Li, T.H., and Hernandez, J.L. “Coherent EEG indicators
of cognitive binding during ambigious figure tasks,” Consc. &
Cognition 9:66–85 (2000).
• Klimesch, W. “EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive
and memory performance: A review and analysis,” Brain Res. Rev.
29:169–195 (1999).
• Knuttinen, M.-G., Power, J.M., Preston, A.R., and Disterhoft, J.F.
“Awareness in classical differential eyeblink conditioning in young and
aging humans,” Behav. Neurosci. 115:747–757 (2001).
• Kobatake, E.,Wang, G., and Tanaka, K. “Effects of shape-discrimination
training on the selectivity of inferotemporal cells in adult monkeys,”
J. Neurophysiol. 80:324–330 (1998).
参考文献43
• Koch, C. “The action of the corticofugal pathway on sensory thalamic
nuclei: A hypothesis,” Neurosci. 23:399–406 (1987).
• Koch, C. “Visual awareness and the thalamic intralaminar nuclei,”
Consc. & Cognition 4:163–165 (1995).
• Koch, C. Biophysics of Computation. New York: Oxford University
Press (1999).
• Koch, C. and Crick, F.C. “Some further ideas regarding the neuronal
basis of awareness.” In: Large-Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain.
Koch, C. and Davis, J., eds., pp. 93–110, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1994).
• Koch, C. and Laurent, G. “Complexity and the nervous system,” Science
284:96–98 (1999).
• Koch, C. and Tootell, R.B. “Stimulating brain but not mind,” Nature
383:301–303 (1996).
• Koch, C. and Ullman, S. “Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards
the underlying neural circuitry,” Human NeuroBiol. 4:219–227
(1985).
• Koffka, K. Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Hartcourt
(1935).
• Kohler, C.G., Ances, B.M., Coleman, A.R., Ragland, J.D., Lazarev,
M., and Gur, R.C. “Marchiafava-Bignami disease: Literature review
and case report,” Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol.
13:67–76 (2000).
• K¨ohler, W. The Task of Gestalt Psychology. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press (1969).
• Kolb, F.C. and Braun, J. “Blindsight in normal observers,” Nature
377:336–338 (1995).
• Komatsu, H., Kinoshita, M., and Murakami, I. “Neural responses in
the retinotopic representation of the blind spot in the macaque V1 to
stimuli for perceptual filling-in,” J. Neurosci. 20:9310–9319 (2000).
• Komatsu, H. and Murakami, I. “Behavioral evidence of filling-in at
the blind spot of the monkey,” Vis. Neurosci. 11:1103–1113 (1994).
参考文献44
• Konorski, J. Integrative Activity of the Brain. Chicago, IL: University
of Chicago Press (1967).
• Kosslyn, S.M. “Visual Consciousness.” In: Finding Consciousness
in the Brain. Grossenbacher P.G., ed., pp. 79–103. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: John Benjamins (2001).
• Kosslyn, S.M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W.L. “Neural foundations
of imagery,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2:635–642 (2001).
• Kosslyn, S.M., Thompson, W.L., and Alpert, N.M. “Neural systems
shared by visual imagery and visual perception: A PET study,” Neuroimage
6:320–334 (1997).
• Koulakov, A.A. and Chklovskii, D.B. “Orientation preference patterns
in mammalian visual cortex: A wire length minimization approach,”
Neuron 29:519–527 (2001).
• Krakauer, J. Eiger Dreams. New York: Lyons & Burford (1990).
• Kreiman, G. On the neuronal activity in the human brain during visual
recognition, imagery and binocular rivalry. Ph.D. Thesis. Pasadena:
California Institute of Technology (2001).
• Kreiman G., Fried, I., and Koch, C. “Single-neuron correlates of subjective
vision in the human medial temporal lobe,” Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. USA 99:8378–8383 (2002).
• Kreiman, G., Koch, C., and Fried, I. “Category-specific visual responses
of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe,” Nature
Neurosci. 3:946–953 (2000a).
• Kreiman, G., Koch, C., and Fried, I. “Imagery neurons in the human
brain,” Nature 408:357–361 (2000b).
• Kreiter, A.K. and Singer, W. “Oscillatory neuronal responses in the visual
cortex of the awake macaque monkey,” Eur. J. Neurosci. 4:369–
375 (1992).
• Kreiter, A.K. and Singer, W. “Stimulus-dependent synchronization of
neuronal responses in the visual cortex of the awake macaque monkey,”
J. Neurosci. 16:2381–2396 (1996).
• Krekelberg, B. and Lappe, M. “Neuronal latencies and the position of
moving objects,” Trends Neurosci. 24:335–339 (2001).
参考文献45
• Kretschmann, H.-J. andWeinrich,W. Cranial Neuroimaging and Clinical
Neuroanatomy. Stuttgart, Germany: Georg Thieme (1992).
• Kristofferson, A.B. “Successiveness discrimination as a two-state, quantal
process,” Science 158:1337–1339 (1967).
• Kuffler, S.W. “Neurons in the retina: Organization, inhibition and excitatory
problems,” Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 17:281–
292 (1952).
• Kulli, J. and Koch, C. “Does anesthesia cause loss of consciousness?”
Trends Neurosci. 14:6–10 (1991).
• Kunimoto, C., Miller, J., and Pashler, H. “Confidence and accuracy of
near-threshold discrimination responses,” Cons. & Cogn. 10:294–340
(2001).
• Kustov, A.A. and Robinson, D.L. “Shared neural control of attentional
shifts and eye movements,” Nature 384:74–77 (1996).
• LaBerge, D. and Buchsbaum, M.S. “Positron emission tomographic
measurements of pulvinar activity during an attention task. J. Neurosci.
10:613–619 (1990).
• Laming, P.R., Sykov´a, E., Reichenbach, A., Hatton, G.I., and Bauer,
H., Glia Cells: Their Role in Behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press (1998).
• Lamme, V.A.F. “Why visual attention and awareness are different,”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:12–18 (2003).
• Lamme, V.A.F. and Roelfsema, P.R. “The distinct modes of vision
offered by feed-forward and recurrent processing,” Trends Neurosci.
23:571–579 (2000).
• Lamme, V.A.F. and Spekreijse, H. “Contextual modulation in primary
visual cortex and scene perception.” In: The New Cognitive
Neurosciences. 2nd ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed., pp. 279–290. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Lamme, V.A.F., Zipser, K., and Spekreijse, H. “Figure-ground activity
in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia,” Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 95:3263–3268 (1998).
• Langston, J.W. and Palfreman, J. The Case of the Frozen Addicts.
New York: Vintage Books (1995).
参考文献46
• Lashley, K.S. “Cerebral organization and behavior.” In: The Brain
and Human Behavior. Proc. Ass. Nervous & Mental Disease, pp.
1–18. New York: Hafner (1956).
• Laurent, G. “A systems perspective on early olfactory coding,” Science
286:723–728 (1999).
• Laurent, G., Stopfer, M., Friedrich, R.W., Rabinovich, M.I., Volkovskii,
A., and Abarbanel, H.D. “Odor encoding as an active, dynamical process:
Experiments, computation, and theory,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci.
24:263–297 (2001).
• Laureys, S., Faymonville, M.E., Degueldre, C., Fiore, G.D., Damas,
P, Lambermont, B., Janssens, N., Aerts, J., Franck, G., Luxen, A.,
Moonen, G., Lamy, M., and Maquet, P. “Auditory processing in the
vegetative state,” Brain 123:1589–1601 (2000).
• Laureys, S., Faymonville, M.E., Peigneux, P., Damas, P., Lambermont,
B., Del Fiore, G., Degueldre, C., Aerts, J., Luxen, A., Franck,
G., Lamy, M., Moonen, G., and Maquet, P. “Cortical processing
of noxious somatosensory stimuli in the persistent vegetative state,”
Neuroimage 17:732–741 (2002).
• Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.F., Poupon, C., Clark, C.A., Pappata, S.,
Molko, N., and Chabriat, H. “Diffusion tensor imaging: Concepts and
applications,” J. Magnetic Resonance Imaging 13:534–546 (2001).
• Lechner, H.A.E., Lein, E.S., and Callaway, E.M. “A genetic method
for selective and quickly reversible silencing of mammalian neurons,”
J. Neurosci. 22:5287–5290 (2002).
• LeDoux, J. The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster
(1996).
• Lee, D.K., Itti, L., Koch, C., and Braun, J. “Attention activates
winner-take-all competition amongst visual filters,” Nature Neurosci.
2:375–381 (1999).
• Lee, D.N. and Lishman, J.R. “Visual proprioceptive control of stance,”
J. Human Movement Studies 1:87–95 (1975).
• Lee, S.-H. and Blake, R. “Rival ideas about binocular rivalry,” Vision
Res. 39:1447–1454 (1999).
参考文献47
• Lehky, S.R. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “No binocular rivalry in the LGN
of alert macaque monkeys,” Vision Res. 36:1225–1234 (1996).
• Lehky, S.R. and Sejnowski, T. J. “Network model of shape-fromshading:
Neural function arises from both receptive and projective
fields”, Nature 333:452–454 (1988).
• Lennie, P. “Color vision.” In: Principles of Neural Science. 4th ed.,
Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H., and Jessel, T.M. eds., pp. 583–599.
New York: McGraw Hill (2000).
• Lennie, P. “The cost of cortical computation,” Current Biol. 13:493–
497 (2003).
• Leopold, D.A. and Logothetis, N.K. “Activity changes in early visual
cortex reflects monkeys’ percepts during binocular rivalry,” Nature
379:549–553 (1996).
• Leopold, D.A. and Logothetis, N.K. “Multistable phenomena: Changing
views in perception,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 3:254–264 (1999).
• Leopold, D.A., Wilke, M., Maier, A., and Logothetis, N.K. “Stable
perception of visually ambiguous patterns,” Nature Neurosci. 5:605–
609 (2002).
• LeVay, S., Connolly, M., Houde, J., and Van Essen, D.C. “The complete
pattern of ocular dominance stripes in the striate cortex and
visual field of the macaque monkey,” J. Neurosci. 5:486–501 (1985).
• LeVay, S. and Gilbert, C.D. “Laminar patterns of geniculocortical
projection in the cat,” Brain Res. 113:1–19 (1976).
• LeVay, S. and Nelson, S.B. “Columnar organization of the visual cortex.”
In: The Neural Basis of Visual Function. Leventhal, A.G., ed.,
pp. 266–314. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (1991).
• Levelt, W. On Binocular Rivalry. Soesterberg, Netherlands: Institute
for Perception RVO-TNO (1965).
• Levick, W.R. and Zacks, J.L. “Responses of cat retinal ganglion cells
to brief flashes of light,” J. Physiol. 206:677–700 (1970).
• Levine, J. “Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap.” Pacific
Philos. Quart. 64:354–361 (1983).
参考文献48
• Levitt, J.B., Kiper, D.C., and Movshon, J.A. “Receptive fields and
functional architecture of macaque V2,” J. Neurophysiol. 71:2517–
2542 (1994).
• Lewis, J.W. and Van Essen, D.C. “Mapping of architectonic subdivisions
in the macaque monkey, with emphasis on parieto-occipital
cortex,” J. Comp. Neurol. 428:79–111 (2000).
• Li, F.F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. “Rapid natural
scene categorization in the near absence of attention,” Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 99:9596–9601 (2002).
• Li, W.H., Parigi, G., Fragai, M., Luchinat, C., and Meade, T.J.
“Mechanistic studies of a calcium-dependent MRI contrast agent,”
Inorg. Chem. 41:4018–4024 (2002).
• Liang, J., Williams, D.R., and Miller, D.T. “Supernormal vision and
high-resolution retinal imaging through adaptive optics,” J. Opt. Soc.
Am. A 14:2884–2892 (1997).
• Libet, B. “Brain stimulation and the threshold of conscious experience.”
In: Brain and Conscious Experience. Eccles, J.C., ed., pp.
165–181. Berlin: Springer (1966).
• Libet, B. “Electrical stimulation of cortex in human subjects and conscious
sensory aspects.” In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol II:
Somatosensory Systems. Iggo, A. ed., pp. 743–790. Berlin: Springer
(1973).
• Libet, B. Neurophysiology of Consciousness: Selected Papers and New
Essays by Benjamin Libet. Boston: Birkh¨auser (1993).
• Lichtenstein, M. “Phenomenal simultaneity with irregular timing of
components of the visual stimulus,” Percept. Mot. Skills 12:47–60
(1961).
• Lisman, J.E. “Bursts as a unit of neural information: Making unreliable
synapses reliable,” Trends Neurosci. 20:38–43 (1997).
• Lisman, J.E. and Idiart, M. A. “Storage of 7±2 short-term memories
in oscillatory subcycles,” Science 267:1512–1515 (1995).
• Livingstone, M.S. “Mechanisms of direction selectivity in macaque
V1,” Neuron 20:509–526 (1998).
参考文献49
• Livingstone, M.S. and Hubel, D.H. “Effects of sleep and rousal on
the processing of visual information in the cat,” Science 291:554–561
(1981).
• Livingstone, M.S. and Hubel, D.H. “Anatomy and physiology of a
color system in the primate visual system,” J. Neurosci. 4:309–356
(1984).
• Livingstone, M.S. and Hubel, D.H. “Connections between layer 4B of
area 17 and thick cytochrome oxidase stripes of area 18 in the squirrel
monkey,” J. Neurosci. 7:3371–3377 (1987).
• Llin´as, R.R. and Par´e, D. “Of dreaming and wakefulness,” Neurosci.
44:521–535 (1991).
• Llin´as, R.R., Ribary, U., Contreras, D., and Pedroarena, C. “The
neuronal basis for consciousness,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B.
Biol. Sci. 353:1841–1849 (1998).
• Loftus, G.R., Duncan, J., and Gehrig, P. “On the time course of
perceptual information that results from a brief visual presentation,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. & Perform. 18:530–549 (1992).
• Logothetis, N.K. “Single units and conscious vision,” Phil. Trans. R.
Soc. Lond. B 353:1801–1818 (1998).
• Logothetis, N.K. “The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent
functional magnetic resonance imaging signal,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc.
Lond. B 357:1003–1037 (2002).
• Logothetis, N.K. “MR imaging in the non-human primate: Studies
of function and dynamic connectivity,” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. in
press (2004).
• Logothetis, N.K., Guggenberger, H., Peled, S., and Pauls, J. “Functional
imaging of the monkey brain,” Nature Neurosci. 2:555–562
(1999).
• Logothetis, N.K., Leopold, D.A., and Sheinberg, D.L. “What is rivalling
during binocular rivalry,” Nature 380:621–624 (1996).
• Logothetis, N.K. and Pauls, J. “Psychophysical and physiological evidence
for viewer-centered object representations in the primate,” Cerebral
Cortex 5:270–288 (1995).
参考文献50
• Logothetis, N.K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., and Oeltermann,
A. “Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal,”
Nature 412:150–157 (2001).
• Logothetis, N.K., Pauls, J., B¨ulthoff, H.H., and Poggio, T. “Viewdependent
object recognition by monkeys,” Curr. Biol. 4:401–414
(1994).
• Logothetis, N.K. and Schall, J.D. “Neuronal correlates of subjective
visual perception,” Science 245:761–763 (1989).
• Logothetis, N.K. and Sheinberg, D.L. “Visual object recognition,”
Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 19:577–621 (1996).
• Louie, K. and Wilson, M.A. “Temporally structured replay of awake
hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep,”
Neuron 29:145–156 (2001).
• Lovibond, P.F. and Shanks, D.R. “The role of awareness in Pavlovian
conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications,”
J. Exp. Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 28:3–26 (2002).
• Lucas, J.R. “Minds, machines and G¨odel,” Philosophy 36:112–127
(1961).
• Luce, R.D. Response Times. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
(1986).
• Luck, S.J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S.A., and Desimone, R. “Neural
mechanisms of spatial attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque
visual cortex,” J. Neurophysiol. 77:24–42 (1997).
• Luck, S.J., Hillyard, S.A., Mangun, G.R., and Gazzaniga, M.S. “Independent
hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in
split-brain patients,” Nature 342:543–545 (1989).
• Luck, S.J., Hillyard, S.A., Mangun, G.R., and Gazzaniga, M.S. “Independent
attentional scanning in the separated hemispheres of splitbrain
patients,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6:84–91 (1994).
• Lumer, E.D., Friston, K.J., and Rees, G. “Neural correlates of perceptual
rivalry in the human brain,” Science 280:1930–1934 (1998).
• Lumer, E.D. and Rees, G. “Covariation of activity in visual and prefrontal
cortex associated with subjective visual perception,” Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96:1669–1673 (1999).
参考文献51
• Lux, S., Kurthen, M., Helmstaedter C., Hartje, W., Reuber, M., and
Elger, C.E. “The localizing value of ictal consciousness and its constituent
functions,” Brain 125:2691–2698 (2002).
• Lyon, D.C. and Kaas, J.H. “Evidence for a modified V3 with dorsal
and ventral halves in macaque monkeys,” Neuron 33:453–461 (2002).
• Lytton, W.W. and Sejnowski, T.J. “Simulations of cortical pyramidal
neurons synchronized by inhibitory interneurons,” J. Neurophysiol.
66:1059–1079 (1991).
• Mack, A. and Rock, I. Inattentional Blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1998).
• Mackintosh, N.J. Conditioning and Associative Learning. Oxford,
UK: Clarendon Press (1983).
• Macknik, S.L. and Livingstone, M.S. “Neuronal correlates of visibility
and invisibility in the primate visual system,” Nat Neurosci. 1:144–
149 (1998).
• Macknik, S.L. and Martinez-Conde, S. “Dichoptic visual masking in
the geniculocortical system of awake primates,” J. Cogn. Neurosci.
in press (2004).
• Macknik, S.L., Martinez-Conde, S., and Haglund, M.M. “The role of
spatiotemporal edges in visibility and visual masking,” Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA. 97:7556–7560 (2000).
• MacLeod, K., Backer, A., and Laurent, G. “Who reads temporal information
contained across synchronized and oscillatory spike trains?”
Nature 395:693–698 (1998).
• MacNeil, M.A. andMasland, R.H. “Extreme diversity among amacrine
cells: Implication for function,” Neuron 20:971–982 (1998).
• Macphail, E.M. The Evolution of Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press (1998).
• Madler, C. and P¨oppel, E. “Auditory evoked potentials indicate the
loss of neuronal oscillations during general anaesthesia,” Naturwissenschaften
74:42–43 (1987).
• Magoun, H.W. “An ascending reticular activating system in the brain
stem,” Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 67:145–154 (1952).
参考文献52
• Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Jung, T.P., Enghoff, S., Townsend, J.,
Courchesne, E., and Sejnowski, T.J. “Dynamic brain sources of visual
evoked responses,” Science 295:690–694 (2002).
• Mandler, G. Consciousness Recovered: Psychological Functions and
Origins of Conscious Thought. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins
(2002).
• Manford, M. and Andermann, F. “Complex visual hallucinations:
Clinical and neurobiological insights,” Brain 121:1819–1840 (1998).
• Mark, V. “Conflicting communicative behavior in a split-brain patient:
Support for dual consciousness.” In: Toward a Science of Consciousness:
The First Tucson Discussions and Debates. Hameroff,
S.R., Kaszniak, A.W., and Scott, A.C., eds., pp. 189–196. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1996).
• Marr, D. Vision. San Francisco, CA: Freeman (1982).(邦訳)乾敏郎,
安藤広志訳,ビジョン: 視覚の計算理論と脳内表現,産業図書(1987).
• Mars´alek, P., Koch, C., and Maunsell, J.H.R. “On the Relationship
between Synaptic Input and Spike Output Jitter in Individual Neurons,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94:735–740 (1997).
• Martinez, J.L. and Kesner, R.P., eds. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory. New York: Academic Press (1998).
• Masand P., Popli, A.P., andWeilburg, J.B. “Sleepwalking,” Am. Fam.
Physician 51:649–654 (1995).
• Masland, R.H. “Neuronal diversity in the retina,” Curr. Opinion
Neurobiol. 11:431–436 (2001).
• Mather, G., Verstraten, F., and Anstis, S. The Motion Aftereffect: A
Modern Perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1998).
• Mathiesen, C., Caesar, K., ¨Oren, N.A., and Lauritzen, M. “Modification
of activity-dependent increases of cerebral blood flow by excitatory
synaptic activity and spikes in rat cerebellar cortex,” J. Physiology
512:555–566 (1998).
• Mattingley, J.B., Husain, M., Rorden, C., Kennard, C., and Driver,
J. “Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral
neglect patients,” Nature 392:179–182 (1998).
参考文献53
• Maunsell, J.H.R. and Van Essen, D.C. “Functional properties of neurons
in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular
interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity,” J. Neurophysiol.
49:1148–1167 (1983).
• McAdams, C.J. andMaunsell, J.H.R. “Effects of attention on orientationtuning
functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4,” J.
Neurosci. 19:431–441 (1999).
• McAdams, C.J. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Attention to both space and
feature modulates neuronal responses in macaque area V4,” J. Neurophysiol.
83:1751–1755 (2000).
• McBain, C.J. and Fisahn, A. “Interneurons unbound,” Nature Rev.
Neurosci. 2:11–23 (2001).
• McClintock, M.K. “Whither menstrual synchrony?” Ann. Rev. Sex
Res. 9:77–95 (1998).
• McComas, A.J. and Cupido, C.M. “The RULER model. Is this how
somatosensory cortex works?” Clinical Neurophysiol. 110:1987–1994
(1999).
• McConkie, G.W. and Currie, C.B. “Visual stability across saccades
while viewing complex pictures,” J. Exp. Psych.: Human Perception
& Performance 22:563–581 (1996).
• McCullough, J.N., Zhang, N., Reich, D.L., Juvonen, T.S., Klein, J.J.,
Spielvogel, D., Ergin, M.A., and Griepp, R.B. “Cerebral metabolic
suppression during hypothermic circulatory arrest in humans,” Ann.
Thorac. Surg. 67:1895–1899 (1999).
• McGinn, C. The Problem of Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Blackwell
(1991).
• McMullin, E. “Biology and the theology of the human.” In: Controlling
Our Destinies. Sloan, P.R., ed., pp. 367–400. Notre Dame, IN:
University of Notre Dame Press (2000).
• Meador, K.J., Ray, P.G., Day, L.J., and Loring, D.W. “Train duration
effects on perception: Sensory deficit, neglect and cerebral lateralization,”
J. Clinical Neurophysiol. 17:406–413 (2000).
• Meadows, J.C. “Disturbed perception of colours associated with localized
cerebral lesions,” Brain 97:615–632 (1974).
参考文献54
• Medina, J.F., Repa, J.C., Mauk, M.D., and LeDoux, J.E. “Parallels
between cerebellum- and amygdala-dependent conditioning,” Nature
Rev. Neurosci. 3:122–131 (2002).
• Meenan, J.P. and Miller, L.A. “Perceptual flexibility after frontal or
temporal lobectomy,” Neuropsychologia 32:1145–1149 (1994).
• Meister, M. “Multineuronal codes in retinal signaling,” Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 93:609–614 (1996).
• Merigan, W.H. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “How parallel are the primate
visual pathways?” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 16:369–402 (1993).
• Merigan, W.H., Nealey, T.A., and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Visual effects of
lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques,” J. Neurosci. 13:3180–3191
(1993).
• Merikle, P.M. “Perception without awareness. Critical issues,” Am.
Psychol. 47:792–795 (1992).
• Merikle, P.M. and Daneman, M. “Psychological investigations of unconscious
perception,” J. Consc. Studies 5:5–18 (1998).
• Merikle, P.M., Smilek, D., and Eastwood, J.D. “Perception without
awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology,” Cognition
79:115–134 (2001).
• Merleau-Ponty, M. The Phenomenology of Perception. C. Smith,
transl., London: Routledge & Kegan Paul (1962).
• Metzinger, T., ed. Conscious Experience. Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic
(1995).
• Metzinger, T., ed. Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and
Conceptual Questions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Michael, C.R. “Color vision mechanisms in monkey striate cortex:
Dual-opponent cells with concentric receptive fields,” J. Neurophysiol.
41:572–588 (1978).
• Michael, C.R. “Columnar organization of color cells in monkey’s striate
cortex,” J. Neurophysiol. 46:587–604 (1981).
• Miller, E.K. “The prefrontal cortex: Complex neural properties for
complex behavior,” Neuron 22:15–17 (1999).
参考文献55
• Miller, E.K. and Cohen, J.D. “An integrative theory of prefrontal
cortex function,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 24:167–202 (2001).
• Miller, E.K., Gochin, P.M., and Gross, C.G. “Suppression of visual
responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque
by addition of a second stimulus,” Brain Res. 616:25–29 (1993).
• Miller, E.K., Erickson, C.A., and Desimone, R. “Neural mechanisms
of visual working memory in prefrontal cortex of the macaque,” J.
Neurosci. 16:5154–5167 (1996).
• Miller, G.A. “The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some
limits on our capacity for processing information,” Psychol. Rev.
63:81–97 (1956).
• Miller, K.D., Chapman, B., and Stryker, M.P. “Visual responses in
adult cat visual cortex depend on N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 86:5183–5187 (1989).
• Miller, S.M., Liu, G.B., Ngo, T.T., Hooper, G., Riek, S., Carson, R.G.,
and Pettigrew, J.D. “Interhemispheric switching mediates perceptual
rivalry,” Curr. Biol. 10:383–392 (2000).
• Millican, P. and Clark, A., eds. Machines and Thought: The Legacy
of Alan Turing. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (1999).
• Milner, A.D. and Dyde, R. “Why do some perceptual illusions affect
visually guided action, when others don’t?” Trends Cogn. Sci. 7:10–
11 (2003).
• Milner, A.D. and Goodale, M.A. The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press (1995).
• Milner, A.D., Perrett, D.I., Johnston, R.S., Benson, P.J., Jordan,
T.R., Heeley, D.W., Bettucci, D., Mortara, F., Mutani, R., Terazzi,
E., and Davidson, D.L.W. “Perception and action in form agnosia,”
Brain 114:405–428 (1991).
• Milner, B. “Disorders of learning and memory after temporal lobe
lesions in man,” Clin. Neurosurg. 19:421–446 (1972).
• Milner, B., Squire, L.R., and Kandel, E.R. “Cognitive neuroscience
and the study of memory,” Neuron 20:445–468 (1998).
• Milner, P. “A model for visual shape recognition,” Psychol. Rev.
81:521–535 (1974).
参考文献56
• Minamimoto, T. and Kimura, M. “Participation of the thalamic CMPf
complex in attentional orienting,” J. Neurophysiol. 87:3090–3101
(2002).
• Minsky, M. The Society of Mind. New York: Simon and Schuster
(1985).(邦訳)安西祐一郎訳,心の社会、産業図書(1990).
• Mitchell, J.P., Macrae, C.N., and Gilchrist, I.D. “Working memory
and the suppression of reflexive saccades,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14:95–
103 (2002).
• Miyashita, Y., Okuno, H., Tokuyama, W., Ihara, T., and Nakajima,
K. “Feedback signal from medial temporal lobe mediates visual associative
mnemonic codes of inferotemporal neurons,” Brain Res. Cogn.
Brain Res. 5:81–86 (1996).
• Moldofsky, H., Gilbert, R., Lue, F.A., and MacLean, A.W. “Sleeprelated
violence,” Sleep 18:731–739 (1995).
• Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Moradi, F., Zand-Vakili, A., and Esteky, H.
“Orientation selective adaptation during motion-induced blindness,”
Perception, in press (2004).
• Moore, G.E. Philosophical Studies. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
(1922).
• Moran, J. and Desimone, R. “Selective attention gates visual processing
in extrastriate cortex,” Science 229:782–784 (1985).
• Morris, J.S., Ohman, A., and Dolan, R.J. “A subcortical pathway to
the right amygdala mediating ‘unseen’ fear,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 96:1680–1685 (1999).
• Moruzzi, G. and Magoun, H.W. “Brain stem reticular formation and
activation of the EEG,” EEG Clin. Neurophysiol. 1:455–473 (1949).
• Motter, B.C. “Focal attention produces spatially selective processing
in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing
stimuli,” J. Neurophysiol. 70:909–919 (1993).
• Mountcastle, V.B. “Modality and topographic properties of single
neurons of cat’s somatic sensory cortex,” J. Neurophysiol. 20:408–
434 (1957).
• Mountcastle, V.B. Perceptual Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press (1998).
参考文献57
• Mountcastle, V.B., Andersen, R.A., and Motter, B.C. “The influence
of attentive fixation upon the excitability of light-sensitive neurons of
the posterior parietal cortex,” J. Neurosci. 1:1218–1235 (1981).
• Moutoussis, K. and Zeki, S. “Functional segregation and temporal
hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
264:1407–1415 (1997a).
• Moutoussis, K. and Zeki, S. “A direct demonstration of perceptual
asynchrony in vision,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264:393–399 (1997b).
• Mumford, D. “On the computational architecture of the neocortex. I.
The role of the thalamo-cortical loop,” Biol. Cybernetics 65:135–145
(1991).
• Mumford, D. “Neuronal architectures for pattern-theoretic problems.”
In: Large Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain. Koch, C., and Davis,
J.L., eds, pp. 125–152. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1994).
• Murakami, I., Komatsu, H., and Kinoshita, M. “Perceptual filling-in
at the scotoma following a monocular retinal lesion in the monkey,”
Visual Neurosci. 14:89–101 (1997).
• Murayama, Y., Leopold, D.A., and Logothetis, N.K. “Neural activity
during binocular rivalry in the anesthetized monkey,” Soc. Neurosci.
Abstr. 448.11 (2000).
• Murphy, N. “Human nature: Historical, scientific, and religious issues.”
In: Whatever Happened to the Soul? Scientific and Theological
Portraits of Human Nature. Brown, W.S., Murphy, N., and Malony
H.N., eds., pp. 1–30. Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press (1998).
• Myerson, J., Miezin, F., and Allman, J.M. “Binocular rivalry in macaque
monkeys and humans: A comparative study in perception,” Behav.
Anal. Lett. 1:149–159 (1981).
• Naccache, L., Blandin, E., and Dehaene, S. “Unconscious masked
priming depends on temporal attention,” Psychol. Sci. 13:416–424
(2002).
• Nadel, L. and Eichenbaum, H. “Introduction to the special issue on
place cells,” Hippocampus 9:341–345 (1999).
• Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T., and
Merzenich, M.M. “Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96:6483–6488 (1999).
参考文献58
• Nagel, T. “What is it like to be a bat?” Philosophical Rev. 83:435–450
(1974).
• Nagel, T. “Panpsychism.” In: Mortal Questions. Nagel, T., ed., pp.
181–195. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (1988).
• Nakamura, R.K. and Mishkin, M. “Blindness in monkeys following
non-visual cortical lesions,” Brain Res. 188:572–577 (1980).
• Nakamura, R.K. and Mishkin, M. “Chronic ‘blindness’ following lesions
of nonvisual cortex in the monkey,” Exp. Brain Res. 63:173–184
(1986).
• Nakayama, K. and Mackeben, M. “Sustained and transient components
of focal visual attention,” Vision Res. 29:1631–1647 (1989).
• Nathans, J. “The evolution and physiology of human color vision:
Insights from molecular genetic studies of visual pigments,” Neuron
24:299–312 (1999).
• Naya, Y., Yoshida, M., and Miyashita, Y. “Backward spreading of
memory-retrieval signal in the primate temporal cortex,” Science 291:
661–664 (2001).
• Newman, J.B. “Putting the puzzle together: Toward a general theory
of the neural correlates of consciousness,” J. Consc. Studies 4:47–66
(1997).
• Newsome, W.T., Britten, K.H. and Movshon, J.A. “Neuronal correlates
of a perceptual decision,” Nature 341:52–54 (1989).
• Newsome, W.T., Maunsell, J.H.R., and Van Essen, D.C. “Ventral
posterior visual area of the macaque: Visual topography and areal
boundaries,” J. Comp. Neurol. 252:139–153 (1986).
• Newsome, W.T. and Pare, E.B. “A selective impairment of motion
perception following lesions of the Middle Temporal visual area (MT),”
J. Neurosci. 8:2201–2211 (1988).
• Niebur, E. and Erd˝os, P. “Theory of the locomotion of nematodes:
Control of the somatic motor neurons by interneurons,” Math. Biosci.
118:51–82 (1993).
• Niebur, E., Hsiao, S.S., and Johnson, K.O. “Synchrony: A neuronal
mechanism for attentional selection?” Curr. Opinion Neurobiol. 12:
190–194 (2002).
参考文献59
• Niebur, E. and Koch, C. “A model for the neuronal implementation
of selective visual attention based on temporal correlation among neurons,”
J. Computational Neurosci. 1: 141–158 (1994).
• Niebur, E., Koch, C., and Rosin, C. “An oscillation-based model
for the neuronal basis of attention,” Vision Research 33:2789–2802
(1993).
• Nijhawan, R. “Motion extrapolation in catching,” Nature 370:256–
257 (1994).
• Nijhawan, R. “Visual decomposition of colour through motion extrapolation,”
Nature 386:66–69 (1997).
• Nimchinsky, E.A., Gilissen, E., Allman, J.M., Perl, D.P., Erwin J.M.,
and Hof, P.R. “A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and
great apes,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96:5268–5273 (1999).
• Nirenberg, S., Carcieri, S.M., Jacobs, A.L., and Latham, P.E. “Retinal
ganglion cells act largely as independent encoders,” Nature 411:698–
701 (2001).
• Nishida, S. and Johnston, A. “Marker correspondence, not processing
latency, determines temporal binding of visual attributes,” Curr. Biol.
12:359–368 (2002).
• No¨e, A. Action in Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2004).
• Noesselt, T., Hillyard, S.A., Woldorff, M.G., Schoenfeld, A., Hagner,
T., Jancke, L., Tempelmann, C., Hinrichs, H., and Heinze, H.J.
“Delayed striate cortical activation during spatial attention,” Neuron
35:575–587 (2002).
• Nordby, K. “Vision in a complete achromat: A personal account.”
In: Night Vision: Basic, Clinical and Applied Aspects. Hess, R.F.,
Sharpe, L.T., and Nordby, K., eds., pp. 290–315. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press (1990).
• Norman, R.A., Maynard, E.M., Guillory, K.S., and Warren, D.J.
“Cortical implants for the blind,” IEEE Spectrum 33:54–59 (1996).
• Norretranders, T. The User Illusion. New York: Penguin (1998).
• Nowak, L.G. and Bullier, J. “The timing of information transfer in
the visual system.” In: Extrastriate Cortex in Primates, Vol. 12.



共通テーマ:健康

『意識の探求』の参考文献20-39

参考文献20
• Dennett, D. “Are we explaining consciousness yet?” Cognition 79:221–
237 (2001).
• Dennett, D. “The gift horse of philosophical instruction,” Trends
Cogn. Sci., in press (2004).
• Dennett, D. and Kinsbourne, M. “Time and the observer,” Behavioral
& Brain Sci. 15:183–247 (1992).
• Desimone, R. and Duncan, J. “Neural mechanisms of selective visual
attention,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 18:193–222 (1995).
• Desimone, R., Wessinger M., Thomas, L., and Schneider, W. “Attentional
control of visual perception: Cortical and subcortical mechanisms,”
Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 55:963–971 (1990).
• Destrebecqz, A. and Cleeremans, A. “Can sequence learning be implicit?
New evidence with the process dissociation procedure,” Psychonomic
Bull. Rev. 8:343–350 (2001).
• DeVries, S.H. and Baylor, D.A. “Mosaic arrangement of ganglion
cell receptive fields in rabbit retina,” J. Neurophysiol. 78:2048–2060
(1997).
• DeWeerd, P., Gattass, R., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L.G. “Responses
of cells in monkey visual cortex during perceptual filling-in of
an artificial scotoma,” Nature 377:731–734 (1995).
• DeWeerd, P., Peralta, III M.R., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L.G.
“Loss of attentional stimulus selection after extrastriate cortical lesions
in macaques,” Nature Neurosci. 2:753–758 (1999).
• DeYoe, E.A., Carman, G.J., Bandettini, P., Glickman, S., Wieser, J.,
Cox, R., Miller, D., and Neitz, J. “Mapping striate and extrastriate
visual areas in human cerebral cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
93:2382–2386 (1996).
• DiCarlo, J.J. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Form representation in monkey
inferotemporal cortex is virtually unaltered by free viewing,” Nature
Neurosci. 3:814–821 (2000).
• DiLollo, V., Enns, J.T., and Rensink, R.A. “Competition for consciousness
among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual
processes,” J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129:481–507 (2000).
参考文献21
• Ditterich, J., Mazurek, M.E., and Shadlen, M.N. “Microstimulation of
visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions,” Nature Neurosci.
6:891–898 (2003).
• Di Virgilio, G. and Clarke, S. “Direct interhemisphere visual input to
human speech areas,” Human Brain Map. 5:347–354 (1997).
• Dmytryk, E. On Film Editing: An Introduction to the Art of Film
Construction. Boston: Focal Press (1984).
• Dobelle,W.H. “Artificial vision for the blind by connecting a television
camera to the visual cortex,” Am. Soc. Artificial Internal Organs J.
46:3–9 (2000).
• Dolan, R.J. “Emotion, cognition, and behavior,” Science 298:1191–
1194 (2002).
• Dosher, B. A. and Sperling, G. “A century of human information
processing theory: Vision, attention, memory.” In: Perception and
Cognition at Century’s End. Hochberg J., ed., pp. 201–254. New
York: Academic Press (1998).
• Douglas, R., Koch, C., Mahowald, M., Martin, K., and Suarez, H.
“Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits,” Science 269:981–985
(1995).
• Dow, B.M. “Orientation and color columns in monkey visual cortex,”
Cerebral Cortex 12:1005–1015 (2002).
• Dowling, J.E. The Retina: An Approachable Part of the Brain. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press (1987).
• Doyle, D.A., Cabral, J.M., Pfuetzner, R.A., Kuo, A., Gulbis, J.M.,
Cohen, S.L., Chait, B.T., and MacKinnon, R. “The structure of the
potassium channel: Molecular basis of K+ conduction and selectivity,”
Science 280:69–77 (1998).
• Dragoi, V., Sharma, J., and Sur, M. “Adaptation-induced plasticity of
orientation tuning in adult visual cortex,” Neuron 28:287–298 (2000).
• Dragunow, M. and Faull, R. “The use of c-fos as a metabolic marker in
neuronal pathway tracing,” J. Neurosci. Methods, 29:261–265 (1989).
• Driver, J. and Baylis, G.C. “Attention and visual object segmentation.”
In: The Attentive Brain. Parasurama R., ed., pp. 299–325.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1998).
参考文献22
• Driver, J. and Mattingley, J.B. “Parietal neglect and visual awareness,”
Nature Neurosci. 1:17–22 (1998).
• Drummond, J.C. “Monitoring depth of anesthesia: With emphasis on
the application of the bispectral index and the middle latency auditory
evoked response to the prevention of recall,” Anesthesiology 93:876–
882 (2000).
• Dudai, Y. The Neurobiology of Memory: Concepts, Findings, Trends.
New York: Oxford University Press (1989).
• Duncan, J. “Selective attention and the organization of visual information,”
J. Exp. Psychology: General 113:501–517 (1984).
• Duncan, J. “Converging levels of analysis in the cognitive neuroscience
of visual attention,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353:1307–1317
(1998).
• Duncan, J. “An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal
cortex,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2:820–829 (2001).
• Eagleman, D.M. and Sejnowski, T.J. “Motion integration and postdiction
in visual awareness,” Science 287:2036–2038 (2000).
• Ebner, A., Dinner, D.S., Noachtar, S., and L¨uders, H. “Automatisms
with preserved responsiveness: A lateralizing sign in psychomotor
seizures,” Neurology 45:61–64 (1995).
• Eccles, J.C. “Do mental events cause neural events analogously to the
probability fields of quantum mechanics?” Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B
227:411–428 (1986).
• Eccles, J.C. Evolution of the Brain: Creation of the Self. London:
Routledge (1988).(邦訳)伊藤正男訳,脳の進化,東京大学出版会(1990).
• Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Jordan, W., Brosch, M., Kruse, W., Munk,
M., and Reitb¨ock, H.J. “Coherent oscillations: a mechanism of feature
linking in the visual cortex?” Biol. Cybern. 60:121–130 (1988).
• Eckhorn, R., Frien, A., Bauer, R., Woelbern, T., and Kehr, H. “High
frequency (60–90 Hz) oscillations in primary visual cortex of awake
monkey,” Neuroreport 4:243–246 (1993).
• Edelman, G.M. The Remembered Present: A Biological Theory of
Consciousness. New York: Basic Books (1989).
参考文献23
• Edelman, G.M. “Naturalizing consciousness: A theoretical framework,”
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100:5520–5524 (2003).
• Edelman, G.M. and Tononi, G. A Universe of Consciousness. New
York: Basic Books (2000).
• Efron, R. “The duration of the present,” Annals New York Acad. Sci.
138:713–729 (1967).
• Efron, R. “The minimum duration of a perception,” Neuropsychologia
8:57–63 (1970a).
• Efron, R. “The relationship between the duration of a stimulus and
the duration of a perception,” Neuropsychologia 8:37–55 (1970b).
• Efron, R. “An invariant characteristic of perceptual systems in the
time domain,” Attention and Performance 4:713–736 (1973a).
• Efron, R. “Conservation of temporal information by perceptual systems,”
Perception & Psychophysics 14:518–530 (1973b).
• Egeth, H.E. and Yantis, S. “Visual attention: Control, representation,
and time course,” Ann. Rev. Psychol. 48:269–297 (1997).
• Eichenbaum, H. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory. New York:
Oxford University Press (2002).
• Ekstrom, A.D., Kahana, M.J., Caplan, J.B., Fields, T.A., Isham,
E.A., Newman, E.L., and Fried, I. “Cellular networks underlying human
spatial navigation,” Nature 425:184–188 (2003).
• Elger, C.E. “Semeiology of temporal lobe seizures.” In: Intractable
Focal Epilepsy. Oxbury, J., Polkey, C.E., and Duchowny, M., eds., pp.
63–68. Philadelphia: Saunders (2000).
• Eliasmith, C. How Neurons Mean: A Neurocomputational Theory of
Representational Content. Ph.D. Dissertation, Dept. of Philosophy,
Washington University, St. Louis, MO (2000).
• Ellenberger, H.F. The Discovery of the Unconscious. NewYork: Basic
Books (1970).
• Elston, G.N. “Pyramidal cells of the frontal lobe: All the more spinous
to think with,” J. Neurosci. 20:RC95 (1–4) (2000).
参考文献24
• Elston, G.N. and Rosa, M.G.P. “The occipitoparietal pathway of
the macaque monkey: Comparison of pyramidal cell morphology in
layer III of functionally related cortical visual areas,” Cerebral Cortex
7:432–452 (1997).
• Elston, G.N. and Rosa, M.G.P. “Morphological variation of layer III
pyramidal neurones in the occipitotemporal pathway of the macaque
monkey visual cortex,” Cerebral Cortex 8:278–294 (1998).
• Elston, G.N., Tweedale, R., and Rosa, M.G.P. “Cortical integration in
the visual system of the macaque monkey: Large-scale morphological
differences in the pyramidal neurons in the occipital, parietal and
temporal lobes,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266:1367–1374 (1999).
• Engel, A.K., Fries, P., K¨onig, P., Brecht, M., and Singer, W. “Temporal
binding, binocular rivalry, and consciousness,” Consc. & Cognition
8:128–151 (1999).
• Engel, S.A., Glover, G.H., and Wandell, B.A. “Retinotopic organization
in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional
MRI,” Cerebral Cortex 7:181–192 (1997).
• Engel, A.K., K¨onig, P., Gray, C.M., and Singer, W. “Stimulus-dependent
neuronal oscillations in cat visual cortex: Inter-columnar interaction
as determined by cross-correlation analysis,” Eur. J. Neurosci. 2:588–
606 (1990).
• Engel, A.K., K¨onig, P., Kreiter, A.K., and Singer, W. “Interhemispheric
synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses in cat visual
cortex,” Science 252:1177–1179 (1991).
• Engel, A.K. and Singer, W. “Temporal binding and the neural correlates
of sensory awareness,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 5:16–25 (2001).
• Engel, S.A., Zhang, X., and Wandell, B.A. “Colour tuning in human
visual cortex measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging,”
Nature 388:68–71 (1997).
• Enns, J.T. and DiLollo, V. “What’s new in visual masking,” Trends
Cogn. Sci. 4:345–352 (2000).
• Enroth-Cugell, C. and Robson, J.G. “Functional characteristics and
diversity of cat retinal ganglion cells,” Inv. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.
25:250–267 (1984).
参考文献25
• Epstein, R. and Kanwisher, N. “A cortical representation of the local
visual environment,” Nature 392:598–601 (1998).
• Ermentrout, B.G. and Kleinfeld, D. “Traveling electrical waves in cortex:
Insights form phase dynamics and speculation on a computational
role,” Neuron 29:33–44 (2001).
• Fahle, M. “Figure-ground discrimination from temporal information,”
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 254:199–203 (1993).
• Farah, M.J. Visual Agnosia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1990).
• Farber, I. and Churchland, P.S. “Consciousness and the neurosciences:
Philosophical and theoretical issues.” In: The Cognitive Neurosciences.
1st ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed., pp. 1295–1306. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1995).
• Fearing, F. Reflex Action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1970).
• Feldman, M.H. “Physiological observations in a chronic case of lockedin
syndrome,” Neurol. 21:459–478 (1971).
• Felleman, D.J. and Van Essen, D.C. “Distributed hierarchical processing
in the primate cerebral cortex,” Cerebral Cortex 1:1–47 (1991).
• Fendt, M. and Fanselow, M.S. “The neuroanatomical and neurochemical
basis of conditioned fear,” Neurosci. & Biobehavioral Rev. 23:743–
760 (1999).
• Ffytche, D.H., Guy, C.N., and Zeki, S. “Motion specific responses
from a blind hemifield,” Brain 119:1971–1982 (1996).
• Ffytche, D.H., Howard, R.J., Brammer, M.J., David, A., Woodruff,
P., and Williams, S. “The anatomy of conscious vision: An fMRI
study of visual hallucinations,” Nature Neurosci. 1:738–742 (1998).
• Finger, S. Origins of Neuroscience. New York: Oxford University
Press (1994).
• Fiorani, M. Jr., Rosa, M.G.P., Gattass, R., and Rocha-Miranda, C.E.
“Dynamic surrounds of receptive fields in primate striate cortex: A
physiological basis for perceptual completion?” Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. USA 89:8547–8551 (1992).
• Flaherty, M.G. A Watched Pot: How We Experience Time. New
York: University Press (1999).
参考文献26
• Flanagan, O. Consciousness Reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1992).
• Flanagan, O. Dreaming Souls. New York: Oxford University Press
(2000).
• Flanagan, O. The Problem of the Soul. New York: Basic Books (2002).
• Flohr, H. “NMDA receptor-mediated computational processes and
phenomenal consciousness.” In: Neural Correlates of Consciousness:
Empirical and Conceptual Questions. Metzinger, T., ed., pp. 245–258.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Flohr, H., Glade, U., and Motzko, D. “The role of the NMDA synapse
in general anesthesia,” Toxicology Lett. 100:23–29 (1998).
• Foote, S.L., Aston-Jones, G., and Bloom, F.E. “Impulse activity of locus
coeruleus neurons in awake rats and monkeys is a function of sensory
stimulation and arousal,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 77:3033–
3037 (1980).
• Foote, S.L. and Morrison, J.H. “Extrathalamic modulation of cortical
function,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 10:67–95 (1987).
• Forster, E.M. and Whinnery, J.E. “Recovery from Gz-induced loss
of consciousness: Psychophysiologic considerations,” Aviation, Space,
Env. Med. 59:517–522 (1988).
• Frank, L.M., Brown, E.N., and Wilson, M. “Trajectory encoding in
the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex,” Neuron 27:169–178 (2000).
• Franks, N.P. and Lieb, W.R. “Molecular and cellular mkechanisms of
general anesthesia,” Nature 367:607–614 (1994).
• Franks, N.P. and Lieb, W.R. “The molecular basis of general anesthesia:
Current ideas.” In: Toward a Science of Consciousness II.
Hameroff, S.R., Kaszniak, A.W., and Scott, A.C., eds., pp.443–457.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1998).
• Franks, N.P. and Lieb, W.R. “The role of NMDA receptors in consciounsess:
What can we learn from anesthetic mechanisms?” In:
Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions.
Metzinger, T., ed., pp. 265–269. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(2000).
参考文献27
• Franz, V.H., Gegenfurtner, K.R., B¨ulthoff, H.H., and Fahle, M. “Grasping
visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception
and action,” Psychol. Sci. 11:20–25 (2000).
• Freedman, D.J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Miller, E.K. “Categorical
representation of visual stimuli in the primate prefrontal cortex,”
Science 291:312–316 (2001).
• Freedman, D.J., Riesenhuber, M., Poggio, T., and Miller, E.K. “Visual
categorization and the primate prefrontal cortex: Neurophysiology
and behavior,” J. Neurophysiol. 88:929–941 (2002).
• Freeman, W.J. Mass Action in the Nervous System. New York: Academic
Press (1975).
• Freud, S. “Das Unbewusste,” Int. Zeitschrift Psychoanal. 3(4):189–
203 and 3(5):257–269 (1915).
• Freud, S. The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works
of Sigmund Freund, Vol. 1: 1886–1899. Strachey, J., ed., London:
The Hogart Press (1966).
• Freund, T.F. and Buzs´aki, G. “Interneurons in the hippocampus,”
Hippocampus 6:347–470 (1996).
• Fried, I. “Auras and experiental responses arising in the temporal
lobe.” In: The Neuropsychiatry of Limbic and Subcortical Disorders.
Salloway S., Malloy P., and Cummings J.L., eds., pp. 113–122. Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Press (1997).
• Fried, I., Wilson, C.L., MacDonald, K.A., and Behnke, E.J. “Electric
current stimulates laughter,” Nature 391:650 (1998).
• Friedman-Hill, S., Maldonado, P.E., and Gray, C.M. “Dynamics of
striate cortical activity in the alertmacaque: I. Incidence and stimulusdependence
of gamma-band neuronal oscillations,” Cerebral Cortex
10:1105–1116 (2000).
• Fries, P., Neuenschwander, S., Engel, A.K., Goebel, R., and Singer,W.
“Rapid feature selective neuronal synchronization through correlated
latency shifting,” Nature Neurosci. 4:194–200 (2001a).
• Fries, P., Reynolds, J.H., Rorie, A.E., and Desimone, R. “Modulation
of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention,”
Science 291:1560–1563 (2001b).
参考文献28
• Fries, P., Schr¨oder, J.-H., Singer, W., and Engel, A.K. “Conditions
of perceptual selection and suppression during interocular rivalry in
strabismic and normal cats,” Vision Res. 41:771–783 (2001c).
• Fries, W. “Pontine projection from striate and prestriate visual cortex
in the macaque monkey: An anterograde study,” Vis. Neurosci.
4:205–216 (1990).
• Fries, P., Roelfsema, P.R., Engel, A.K., K¨onig, P., and Singer, W.
“Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates
with perception in interocular rivalry,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
94:12699–12704 (1997).
• Frith, C.D. “The role of prefrontal cortex in self-consciousness: The
case of auditory hallucinations,” Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B
351:1505–1512 (1996).
• Fuster, J.M. “Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response
performance: Neuronal correlates of transient memory,” J. Neurophysiol.
36:61–78 (1973).
• Fuster, J.M. Memory in the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1995).
• Fuster, J.M. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology
of the Frontal Lobe. 3rd ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott-
Raven (1997).
• Fuster, J.M. “Executive frontal functions,” Exp. Brain Res. 133:66–
70 (2000).
• Gail, A., Brinksmeyer, H.J., and Eckhorn, R. “Perception-related
modulations of local field potential power and coherence in primary
visual cortex of awakemonkey during binocular rivalry,” Cerebral Cortex,
in press (2004).
• Galambos, R., Makeig, S., and Talmachoff, P.J. “A 40-Hz auditory
potential recorded from the human scalp,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
78:2643–2647 (1981).
• Galin, D. “The structure of awareness: Contemporary applications of
William James’ forgotten concept of ‘the fringe’,” J. Mind & Behavior
15:375–402 (1997).
参考文献29
• Gallant, J.L., Connor, C.E., and Van Essen, D.C. “Neural activity in
areas V1, V2 and V4 during free viewing of natural scenes compared
to controlled viewing,” Neuroreport 9:2153–2158 (1997).
• Gallant, J.L., Shoup, R.E., and Mazer, J.A. “A human extrastriate
area functionally homologous to macaque V4,” Neuron 27:227–235
(2000).
• Gallistel, C.R. The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1990).
• Gandhi, S.P., Heeger, D.J., and Boynton, G.M. “Spatial attention
affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex,” Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 96:3314–3319 (1999).
• Gangestad, S.W., Thornhill, R., and Garver, C.E. “Changes in women’s
sexual interests and their partners’ mate-retention tactics across the
menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest,” Proc. Roy.
Soc. Lond. B 269:975–982 (2002).
• Gawne, T.J. and Martin, J.M. “Activity of primate V1 cortical neurons
during blinks,” J. Neurophysiol. 84:2691–2694 (2000).
• Gazzaniga, M.S. “Principles of human brain organization derived from
split-brain studies,” Neuron 14:217–228 (1995).
• Gegenfurtner, K. R. and Sperling, G. “Information transfer in iconic
memory experiments,” J. Exp. Psychol. 19:845–866 (1993).
• Geissler, H.G., Schebera, F.U., and Kompass, R. “Ultra-precise quantal
timing: evidence from simultaneity thresholds in long-range apparent
movement,” Percept. Psychophys. 61:707–726 (1999).
• Gershon, M.D. The Second Brain: The Scientific Basis of Gut Instinct.
New York: Harper Collins (1998).
• Geschwind, N. and Galaburda, A.M. Cerebral Laterization. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1987).
• Gho, M. and Varela, F.J. “A quantitative assessment of the dependency
of the visual temporal frame upon the cortical rhythm,” J.
Physiol. Paris 83:95–101 (1988).
• Ghose, G.M. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Attentional modulation in visual
cortex depends on task timing,” Nature 419:616–620 (2002).
参考文献30
• Giacino, J.T. “Disorders of consciousness: Differential diagnosis and
neuropathologic features,” Seminars Neurol. 17:105–111 (1997).
• Gibson, J.J. The Senses Considered as a Perceptual System. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin (1966).
• Gibson, J.R., Beierlein, M., and Connors, B.W. “Two networks of
electrically coupled inhibitory neurons in neocortex,” Nature 402:75–
79 (1999).
• Gladwell, M. “Wrong turn,” The New Yorker, June 11, 50–61 (2001).
• Glickstein, M. “How are visual areas of the brain connected to motor
areas for the sensory guidance of movement?” Trends Neurosci.
23:613–617 (2000).
• Gloor, P. “Consciousness as a neurological concept in epileptology: A
critical review,” Epilepsia 27 (Suppl 2):S14–S26 (1986).
• Gloor, P., Olivier A., and Ives J. “Loss of consciousness in temporal
lobe seizures: Observations obained with stereotaxic depth electrode
recordings and stimulations.” In: Adv. in Epileptology: 11th Epilepsy
Intl. Symposium. Canger, R., Angeleri, F., and Penry, J.K., eds., pp.
349–353. New York: Raven Press (1980).
• Goebel, R., Khorram-Sefat, D., Muckli, L., Hacker, H., and Singer,W.
“The constructive nature of vision: Direct evidence from functional
magnetic resonance imaging studies of apparent motion and motion
imagery,” Eur. J. Neurosci. 10:1563–1573 (1998).
• Gold, J.L. and Shadlen, M.N. “Banburismus and the brain: Decoding
the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward,”
Neuron 36:299–308 (2002).
• Goldberg, E. The Executive Brain: Frontal Lobes and the Civilized
Mind. New York: Oxford University Press (2001).
• Goldman-Rakic, P.S. “Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the
central executive,” Annals New York Acad. Sci. 769:71–83 (1995).
• Goldman-Rakic, P.S., Scalaidhe, S.P.O., and Chafee, M.W. “Domain
specificity in cognitive systems.” In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences.
2nd ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed., pp. 733–742. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (2000).
参考文献31
• Goldstein, K. and Gelb, A. “Psychologische Analysen hirnpathologischer
F¨alle auf Grund von Untersuchungen Hirnverletzter. I Zur Psychologie
des optische Wahr-nehmungs-und Erkennungsvorganges,” Z.
Neurologie & Psychiatrie 41:1–142 (1918).
• Goodale, M.A. “Perception and action in the human visual system.”
In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd ed., Gazzaniga, M.S., ed.,
pp. 365–377. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Goodale, M.A., Jakobson, L.S., and Keillor, J.M. “Differences in the
visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements,” Neuropsychologia
32:1159–1178 (1994).
• Goodale, M.A. and Milner, A.D. Sight Unseen. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press (2004).
• Goodale, M.A., P´elisson, D., and Prablanc, C. “Large adjustments
in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or
perception of target displacement,” Nature 320:748–750 (1986).
• Gordon, H. W. and Bogen, J.E. “Hemispheric lateralization of singing
after intracarotid sodium amylobarbitone,” J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiat. 37:727–738 (1974).
• Gottlieb, J.P., Kusunoki, M., and Goldberg, M.E. “The representation
of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex,” Nature 391:481–484
(1998).
• Gowdy, P.D., Stromeyer, C.F. III, and Kronauer, R.E. “Detection of
flickering edges: Absence of a red-green edge detector,” Vision Res.
39:4186–4191 (1999).
• Grafman, J., Holyoak, K.J., and Boller, F., eds. Structure and Function
of the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Annals New York Acad. Sci.
769 (1995).
• Granon, S., Faure, P., and Changeux, J.P. “Executive and social behaviors
under nicotinic receptor regulation,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 100:9596–9601 (2003).
• Gray, C.M. “The temporal correlation hypothesis of visual feature
integration: Still alive and well,” Neuron 24:31–47 (1999).
• Gray, C.M., K¨onig, P., Engel, A.K., and Singer, W. “Oscillatory responses
in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization
which reflects global stimulus properties,” Nature 338:334–337 (1989).
参考文献32
• Gray, C.M. and Singer, W. “Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in
orientation columns of cat visual cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 86:1698–1702 (1989).
• Graziano, M.S.A., Taylor, C.R.S., and Moore, T. “Complex movements
evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex,” Neuron 34:841–
851 (2002).
• Greenfield, S.A. Journeys to the Centers of the Mind. Toward a Science
of Consciousness. New York: W.H. Freeman (1995).
• Gregory, R.L. “Cognitive contours,” Nature 238:51–52 (1972).
• Gregory, R.L. Eye and Brain: The Psychology of Seeing. 5th ed.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (1997).
• Grieve, K.L., Acuna, C., and Cudeiro, J. “The primate pulvinar nuclei:
Vision and action,” Trends Neurosci. 23:35–38 (2000).
• Griffin, D.R. Animal Minds: Beyond Cognition to Consciousness.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press (2001).(邦訳)長野敬, 宮
木陽子訳,動物の心,青土社(1995).
• Griffin, D.R. and Speck, G.B. “New evidence of animal consciousness,”
Animal Cognition, in press (2004).
• Grimes, J. “On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades.”
In: Perception (Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science, Vol.
2). Akins, K., ed., pp. 89–110. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
(1996).
• Gross, C.G. Brain, Vision, Memory: Tales in the History of Neuroscience.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1998).
• Gross, C.G. “Genealogy of the ‘Grandmother cell’, ” Neuroscientist
8:512–518 (2002).
• Gross, C.G., Bender, D.B., and Rocha-Miranda, C.E. “Visual receptive
fields of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the monkey,” Science
166:1303–1306 (1969).
• Gross, C.G. and Graziano, M.S.A. “Multiple representations of space
in the brain,” Neuroscientist 1:43–50 (1995).
• Gross, C.G., Rocha-Miranda C.E., and Bender D.B. “Visual properties
of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the macaque,” J. Neurophysiol.
35:96–111 (1972).
参考文献33
• Grossberg, S. “The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness,”
Conscious. Cogn. 8:1–44 (1999).
• Grossenbacher, P.G. and Lovelace, C.T. “Mechanisms of synaesthesia:
Cognitive and physiological constraints,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 5:36–41
(2001).
• Grossmann, R.G. “Are current concepts and methods in neuroscience
inadequate for studying the neural basis of consciosuness and mental
activity?” In: Information Processing in the Nervous System, Pinsker,
H.M. and Willis, W.D. Jr., eds. New York: Raven Press (1980).
• Grunewald, A., Bradley, D.C., and Andersen, R.A. “Neural correlates
of structure-from-motion perception in macaque V1 and MT,”
J. Neurosci. 22:6195–6207 (2002).
• Grush, R. and Churchland, P.S. “Gaps in Penrose’s toiling,” J. Consc.
Studies 2:10–29 (1995).
• Gr¨usser, O.J. and Landis, T. Visual Agnosias and Other Disturbances
of Visual Perception and Cognition. Houndmills, UK: MacMillan
Press (1991).
• Guilleminault, C. “Cataplexy.” In: Narcolepsy. Guilleminault, C.,
Dennet, W.C., and Passouant, P. eds., pp. 125–143. New York: Spectrum
(1976).
• Gur, M. and Snodderly, D.M. “A dissociation between brain activity
and perception: Chromatically opponent cortical neurons signal chromatic
flicker that is not perceived,” Vision Res. 37:377–382 (1997).
• Haarmeier, T., Thier, P., Repnow, M., and Petersen, D. “False perception
of motion in a patient who cannot compensate for eye movements,”
Nature 389:849–852 (1997).
• Hadamard, J. The Mathematician’s Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press (1945).
• Hadjikhani, N., Liu, A.K., Dale, A.M., Cavanagh, P., and Tootell,
R.B. “Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area
V8,” Nature Neurosci. 1:235–241 (1998).
• Hahnloser, R.H.R., Kozhevnikov, A.A., and Fee, M.S. “An ultrasparse
code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird,”
Nature 419:65–70 (2002).
参考文献34
• Haines, R.F. “A breakdown in simultaneous information processing.”
In: Presbyopia Research: From Molecular Biology to Visual Adaptation.
Obrecht, G. and Stark, L., eds., pp. 171–175. New York:
Plenum Press (1991).
• Hameroff, S.R. and Penrose, R. “Orchestrated reduction of quantum
coherence in brain microtubules: A model for consciousness.” In:
Toward a Science of Consciousness. Hameroff, S.R., Kaszniak, A.W.,
and Scott, A.C., eds., pp. 507–540. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1996).
• Hamker, F.H. “A dynamic model of how feature cues guide spatial
attention,” Vision Res., in press (2004).
• Hamker F.H. and Worcester, J. “Object detection in natural scenes
by feedback.” In: Biologically Motivated Computer Vision. Lecture
Notes in Computer Science. B¨uelthoff, H.H., ed., pp. 398–407. Berlin:
Springer (2002).
• Han, C.J., O’Tuathaigh, C.M., van Trigt, L., Quinn, J.J., Fanselow,
M.S., Mongeau, R., Koch, C., and Anderson, D.J. “Trace but not
delay fear conditioning requires attention and the anterior cingulate
cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 100:13087–13092 (2003).
• Hardcastle, V.G. “Attention versus consciousness.” In: Neural Basis
of Consciousness. Osaka N., ed., pp. 105–121. Amsterdam, Netherlands:
John Benjamins (2003).
• Hardin, C.L. Color for Philosophers: Unweaving the Rainbow. Indianapolis,
IN: Hackett Publishing Company (1988).
• Harris, K.D., Csicsvar, J., Hirase, H., Dragoi, G., and Buzs´aki, G.
“Organization of cell assembles in the hippocampus,” Nature 424:552–
556 (2003).
• Harrison, R.V., Harel, N., Panesar, J., and Mount, R.J. “Blood capillary
distribution correlates with hemodynamic-based functional imaging
in cerebral cortex,” Cerebral Cortex 12:225–233 (2002).
• Harter, M.R., “Excitability cycles and cortical scanning: A review of
two hypotheses of central intermittency in perception,” Psychol. Bull.
68:47–58 (1967).
参考文献35
• Haxby, J.V., Gobbini, M.I., Furey, M.L., Ishai, A., Schouten, J.L., and
Pietrini, P. “Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and
objects in ventral temporal cortex,” Science 293:2425–2430 (2001).
• Haxby, J.V., Hoffman, E.A., and Gobbini, M.I. “The distributed human
neural system for face perception,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 4:223–233
(2000).
• He, S., Cavanagh, P., and Intrilligator, J. “Attentional resolution and
the locus of visual awareness,” Nature 383:334–337 (1996).
• He, S., Cohen, E.R., and Hu, X. “Close correlation between activity in
brain area MT/V5 and the perception of a visual motion aftereffect,”
Curr. Biol. 8:1215–1218 (1998).
• He, S. and MacLeod, D.I.A. “Orientation-selective adaptation and tilt
aftereffect from invisible patterns,” Nature 411:473–476 (2001).
• Hebb, D.O. The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory.
New York: Wiley (1949).
• Heeger, D.J., Boynton, G.M., Demb, J.B., Seideman, E., and Newsome,
W.T. “Motion opponency in visual cortex,” J. Neurosci. 19:7162–
7174 (1999).
• Heeger, D.J., Huk, A.C., Geisler, W.S., and Albrecht, D.G. “Spikes
versus BOLD: What does neuroimaging tell us about neuronal activity,”
Nature Neurosci. 3:631–633 (2000).
• Heilman, K.M.,Watson, R.T., and Valenstein, E. “Neglect and related
disorders.” In: Clinical Neuropsychology. 4th ed., Heilman, K.M. and
Valenstein, E., eds., pp. 296–346. New York: Oxford University Press
(2003).
• Heinemann, S.H., Terlau, H., St¨uhmer, W., Imoto, K., and Numa, S.
“Calcium-channel characteristics conferred on the sodium-channel by
single mutations,” Nature 356:441–443 (1992).
• Heisenberg, M. and Wolf, R. Vision in Drosophila: Genetics of Microbehavior.
Studies in Brain Function, Vol. 12. Heidelberg, Germany:
Springer (1984).
• Herrigel, E. Zen in the Art of Archery. New York: Pantheon Books
(1953).
参考文献36
• Herzog, M. and Koch, C. “Seeing properties of an invisible object:
Feature inheritance and shine-through,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
98:4271–4275 (2001).
• Herzog, M., Parish, L., Koch, C., and Fahle, M. “Fusion of competing
features is not serial,” Vision Res. 43:1951–1960 (2003).
• Hess, R.H., Baker, C.L., and Zihl, J. “The motion-blind patient: Lowlevel
spatial and temporal filters,” J. Neurosci. 9:1628–1640 (1989).
• Heywood, C.A. and Zihl, J. “Motion blindness.” In: Case Studies
in the Neuropsychology of Vision. Humphreys, G.W., ed., pp. 1–16.
Psychology Press (1999).
• Hilgetag, C.-C., O’Neill, M.A., and Young, M.P. “Indeterminate organization
of the visual system,” Science 271: 776–777 (1996).
• Hille, B. Ionic Channels of Excitable Membranes. 3rd ed. Sunderland,
MA: Sinauer Associates: (2001).
• Hirsh, I.J. and Sherrick, C.E. “Perceived order in different sense modalities,”
J. Exp. Psychol. 62:423–432 (1961).
• Hobson, J.A. Sleep. New York: Scientific American Library, Freeman
(1989).
• Hobson, J.A. Consciousness. New York: Scientific American Library,
Freeman (1999).
• Hobson, J.A., Stickgold, R., and Pace-Schott, E.F. “The neurophysiology
of REM sleep dreaming,” Neuroreport 9:R1–R14 (1998).
• Hochstein, S. and Ahissar, M. “View from the top: Hierarchies and
reverse hierarchies in the visual system,” Neuron 36:791–804 (2002).
• Hofst¨otter, C., Koch, C., and Kiper, D.C. “Absence of high-level contributions
to the formation of afterimages,” Soc. Neurosci. Abstr.,
819:24 (2003).
• Holender, D. “Semantic activation without conscious identification in
dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey
and appraisal,” Behav. Brain Sci. 9:1–23 (1986).
• Holt, G.R. and Koch, C. “Electrical interactions via the extracellular
potential near cell bodies,” J. Computat. Neurosci. 6:169–184 (1999).
参考文献37
• Holy, T.E., Dulac, C., and Meister, M. “Responses of vomeronasal
neurons to natural stimuli,” Science 289:1569–1572 (2000).
• Horgan, J. The End of Science. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley (1996).
• Horton, J.C. and Hedley-Whyte, E.T. “Mapping of cytochrome oxidase
patches and ocular dominance columns in human visual cortex,”
Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 304:255–272 (1984).
• Horton, J.C. and Hoyt, W.F. “The representation of the visual field
in human striate cortex,” Arch. Opthalmology 109:816–824 (1991a).
• Horton, J.C. and Hoyt, W.F. “Quadratic visual field defects: A hallmark
of lesions in extrastriate (V2/V3) cortex,” Brain 114:1703–1718
(1991b).
• Hu, Y. and Goodale, M.A. “Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling
from absolute to relative metrics,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12:856–868
(2000).
• Hubel, D.H. Eye, Brain, and Vision. New York: Scientific American
Library (1988).
• Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. “Receptive fields of single neurons in
the cat’s striate cortex,” J. Physiol. 148:574–591 (1959).
• Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. “Receptive fields, binocular interaction
and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex,” J. Physiol.
160:106–154 (1962).
• Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. “Receptive fields and functional architecture
of monkey striate cortex,” J. Physiol. 195:215–243 (1968).
• H¨ubener M., Shoham, D., Grinvald, A., and Bonhoeffer, T. “Spatial
relationships among three columnar systems in cat area 17,” J. Neurosci.
17:9270–9284 (1997).
• Huerta, M.F., Krubitzer, L.A., and Kaas, J.H. “Frontal eye field as defined
by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys
and macaque monkeys: I. Subcortical connections,” J. Comp.
Neurol. 253:415–439 (1986).
• Huk, A.C., Ress, D., and Heeger, D.J. “Neuronal basis of the motion
aftereffect reconsidered,” Neuron 32:161–172 (2001).
参考文献38
• Hunter, J. and Jasper, H.H. “Effects of thalamic stimulation in unanesthetized
cats,” EEG Clin. Neurophysiol. 1:305–315 (1949).
• Hupe, J.M., James, A.C., Payne, B.R., Lomber, S.G., Girard, P., and
Bullier, J. “Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure
and background by V1, V2, and V3 neurons,” Nature 394:784–787
(1998).
• Husain, M. and Rorden, C. “Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in
hemispatial neglect,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4:26–36 (2003).
• Huxley, T.H. Animal Automatism, and Other Essays. Humboldt Library
of Popular Science Literature. New York: J. Fitzgerald (1884).
• Ilg, U.J. and Thier, P. “Inability of rhesus monkey area V1 to discriminate
between self-induced and externally induced retinal image
slip,” Eur. J. Neurosci. 8:1156–1166 (1996).
• Inoue, Y. and Mihara, T. “Awareness and responsiveness during partial
seizures,” Epilepsia 39:7–10 (1998).
• Ishai, A., Ungerleider, L.G., Martin, A., and Haxby, J.V. “The representation
of objects in the human occipital and temporal cortex,” J.
Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 2):35–51 (2000).
• Ito, M. and Gilbert, C.D. “Attention modulates contextual influences
in the primary visual cortex of alert monkeys,” Neuron 22:593–604
(1999).
• Ito, M., Tamura, H., Fujita, I., and Tanaka, K. “Size and position
invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex,” J.
Neurophysiol. 73:218–226
(1995).
• Itti, L. and Koch, C. “A saliency-based search mechanism for overt
and covert shifts of visual attention,” Vision Res. 40:1489–1506
(2000).
• Itti, L. and Koch, C. “Computational modeling of visual attention,”
Nature Rev. Neurosci. 2:194–204 (2001).
• Itti, L., Koch, C., and Niebur, E. “A model of saliency-based visual
attention for rapid scene analysis,” IEEE Trans. Pattern Analysis &
Machine Intell. (PAMI) 20:1254–1259 (1998).
参考文献39
• Jackendoff, R. Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1987).
• Jackendoff, R. “How language helps us think,” Pragmatics & Cognition
4:1–34 (1996).
• Jacobson, A., Kales, A., Lehmann, D., and Zweizig, J.R. “Somnambulism:
All-night electroencephalographic studies,” Science 148:975–
977 (1965).
• Jacoby, L.L. “A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic
from intentional uses of memory,” J. Memory Lang. 30:513–541
(1991).
• James, W. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications
(1890).
• James, W. Psychology: Briefer Course. New York: Collier Books
(1962).
• Jameson, K.A., Highnote, S.M., and Wasserman, L.M. “Richer color
experience in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes,” Psychonomic
Bulletin & Rev. 8:244–261 (2001).
• J¨arvilehto, T. “The theory of the organism-environment system: IV.
The problem of mental activity and consciousness,” Int. Physiol. Behav.
Sci. 35:35–57 (2000).
• Jasper, H.H. “Sensory information and conscious experience,” Adv.
Neurol. 77:33–48 (1998).
• Jaynes, J. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral
Mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin (1976).
• Jeannerod, M. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Action. Oxford, UK:
Blackwell (1997).
• Johnson, R.R. and Burkhalter, A. “A polysynaptic feedback circuit
in rat visual cortex,” J. Neurosci. 17:129–140 (1997).
• Johnson-Laird, P.N. “A computational analysis of consciousness,” Cognition
& Brain Theory 6:499–508 (1983).
• Johnston, R.W. “Pheromones, the vomeronasal system, and communication.”
In: Olfaction and Taste XII: An International Symposium.
Murphy, C., ed., pp. 333–348. Annals New York Acad. Sci. 855
(1998).



共通テーマ:健康

『意識の探求』の参考文献1-19

参考文献1
『意識の探求』の参考文献
• Abbott, L.F., Rolls, E.T., and Tovee, M.J. “Representational capacity
of face coding in monkeys,” Cerebral Cortex 6:498–505 (1996).
• Abeles, M. Corticonics: Neural Circuits of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press (1991).
• Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Margalit, E., and Vaadia, E. “Spatiotemporal
firing patterns in the frontal cortex of behaving monkeys,” J.
Neurophysiol. 70:1629–1638 (1993).
• Aboitiz, F., Scheibel, A.B., Fisher, R.S., and Zaidel, E. “Fiber composition
of the human corpus callosum,” Brain Res. 598:143–153
(1992).
• Abrams, R.A. and Landgraf, J.Z. “Differential use of distance and location
information for spatial localization,” Perception & Psychophysics
47:349–359 (1990).
• Achenbach, J. Captured by Aliens: The Search for Life and Truth in
a Very Large Universe. New York: Simon & Schuster (1999).
• Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J.,
Phelps, E.A., Anderson, A., Lee G.P., and Damasio, A.R. “Recognition
of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala
damage,” Neuropsychologia 37:1111–1117 (1999).
• Aglioto, S., DeSouza, J.F.X., and Goodale, M.A. “Size-contrast illusions
deceive the eye but not the hand,” Curr. Biol. 5:679–685
(1995).
• Ahmed, B., Anderson, J., Douglas, R., Martin, K., and Nelson, C.
“Polyneuronal innervation of spiny stellate neurons in cat visual cortex,”
J. Comp. Neurol. 341:39–49 (1994).
• Akelaitis, A.J. “Studies on corpus callosum: Higher visual functions
in each homonymous field following complete section of corpus callosum,”
Arch. Neurol. Psych. (Chicago) 45:788–798 (1941).
• Akelaitis, A.J. “A study of gnosis, praxis and language following section
of the corpus callosum and anterior commisure,” J. Neurosurg.
1:94–102 (1944).
参考文献2
• Aksay, E., Gamkrelidze, G., Seung, H.S., Baker, R., and Tank, D.W.
“In vivo intracellular recording and perturbation of persistent activity
in a neural integrator,” Nature Neurosci. 4:184–193 (2001).
• Alauddin, M.M., Louie, A.Y., Shahinian, A., Meade, T.J., and Conti,
P.S. “Receptor mediated uptake of a radiolabeled contrast agent sensitive
to beta-galactosidase activity,” Nucl. Med. Biol. 30:261–265
(2003).
• Albright, T.D. “Cortical processing of visual motion,” Rev. Oculomot.
Res. 51:77–201 (1993).
• Aldrich, M.S., Alessi, A.G., Beck, R.W., and Gilman, S. “Cortical
blindness: Etiology, diagnosis and prognosis,” Ann. Neurol. 21:149–
158 (1987).
• Alkire, M.T., Haier, R.J., Shah, N.K., and Anderson, C.T. “Positron
emission tomograpy study of regional cerebral metabolism in humans
during isoflurane anesthesia,” Anesthesiology 86:549–557 (1997).
• Alkire, M.T., Pomfrett, C.J.D., Haier, R.J., Gianzero, M.V., Chan,
C.M., Jacobsen, B.P., and Fallon, J.H. “Functional brain imaging
during anesthesia in humans,” Anesthesiology 90:701–709 (1999).
• Allen, W. Getting Even. New York: Random House (1978).
• Allman, J.M. “Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical
receptive field: Neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons
in visual neurons,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 8:407–430 (1985).
• Allman, J.M. Evolving Brains. New York: Scientific American Library
(1999).
• Allman, J.M. and Kaas, J.H. “A representation of the visual field
in the caudal third of the middle temporal gyrus of the owl monkey
(Aotus trivirgatus),” Brain Res. 31:85–105 (1971).
• Anderson, M.C. and Green, C. “Suppressing unwanted memories by
executive control,” Nature 410:366–369 (2001).
• Andersen, R.A. “Neural mechanisms of visual motion perception in
primates,” Neuron 18:865–872 (1997).
• Andersen, R.A. “Encoding of intention and spatial location in the
posterior parietal cortex,” Cerebral Cortex 5:457–469 (1995).
参考文献3
• Andersen, R.A., Asanuma, C., Essick, G., and Siegel, R.M. “Corticocortical
connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions
within the inferior parietal lobule,” J. Comp. Neurol. 296:65–
113 (1990).
• Andersen, R.A, Essick, G., and Siegel, R. “Encoding of spatial location
by posterior parietal neurons,” Science 230:456–458 (1985).
• Andersen, R.A., Snyder L.H., Bradley, D.C., and Xing, J. “Multimodal
representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its
use in planning movements,” Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 20:303–330 (1997).
• Andrews, T.J., Halpern, S.D., and Purves, D. “Correlated size variations
in human visual cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus and optic
tract,” J. Neurosci. 17:2859–2868 (1997).
• Andrews, T.J., and Purves, D. “Similarities in normal and binocularly
rivalrous viewing,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 94:9905–9908 (1997).
• Antkowiak, B. “How do general anesthetics work,” Naturwissenschaften
88:201–213 (2001).
• Arnold, D.H., Clifford, C.W.G., and Wenderoth, P. “Asynchronous
processing in vision: Color leads motion,” Curr. Biol. 11:596–600
(2001).
• Asenjo, A.B., Rim, J., and Oprian, D.D. “Molecular determinants of
human red/green color discrimination,” Neuron 12:1131–1138 (1994).
• Astafiev, S.V., Shulman, G.L., Stanley, C.M., Snyder, A.Z., Van Essen,
D.C., and Corbetta, M. “Functional Organization of Human Intraparietal
and Frontal Cortex for Attending, Looking, and Pointing,”
J. Neurosci. 23:4689–4699 (2003).
• Attneave, F. “In defense of homunculi.” In: Sensory Communication.
Rosenblith W.A., ed., pp. 777–782. New York: MIT Press (1961).
• Baars, B.J. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press (1988).
• Baars, B.J. “Surprisingly small subcortical structures are needed for
the state of waking consciousness, while cortical projection areas seem
to provide perceptual contents of consciousness,” Consc. & Cognition
4:159–162 (1995).
参考文献4
• Baars, B.J. In the Theater of Consciousness. New York: OxfordUniversity
Press (1997).
• Baars, B.J. “The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence,”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 6:47–52 (2002).
• Bachmann, T. Psychophysiology of Visual Masking. Commack, NY:
Nova Science Publishers (1994).
• Bachmann T. Microgenetic Approach to the Conscious Mind. Amsterdam,
Netherlands: Johns Benjamins (2000).
• Baddeley, A. Working Memory. London, UK: Oxford University Press
(1986).
• Baddeley, A. Human Memory: Theory and Practice. Boston: Allyn
& Bacon (1990).
• Baddeley, A. “The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 4:417–423 (2000).
• Baer, P.E., and Fuhrer, M.J. “Cognitive processes in the differential
trace conditioning of electrodermal and vasomotor activity,” J. Exp.
Psychology 84:176–178 (1970).
• Bair,W. “Spike timing in the mammalian visual system,” Curr. Opinion
Neurobiol. 9:447–453 (1999).
• Bair, W. and Koch, C. “Temporal precision of spike trains in extrastriate
cortex of the behaving monkey,” Neural Comp. 8:1185–1202
(1996).
• Baizer, J.A., Ungerleider, L.G., and Desimone, R. “Organization of
visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in
macaques,” J. Neurosci. 11:168–190 (1991).
• Bar, M. and Biederman, I. “Subliminal visual priming,” Psychological
Science 9:464–469 (1998).
• Bar, M. and Biederman, I. “Localizing the cortical region mediating
visual awareness of object identity,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
96:1790–1793 (1999).
• Barbas, H. “Pattern in the laminar origin of corticocortical connections,”
J. Comp. Neurol. 252:415–422 (1986).
参考文献5
• Barcelo, F., Suwazono, S., and Knight, R.T. “Prefrontal modulation
of visual processing in humans,” Nature Neurosci. 3:399–403 (2000).
• Bargmann, C.I. “Neurobiology of the Caenorhabditis elegans genome,”
Science 282:2028–2033 (1998).
• Barlow, H.B. “Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual
psychology,” Perception 1:371–394 (1972).
• Barlow, H.B. “The neuron doctrine in perception.” In: The Cognitive
Neurosciences. 1st ed., Gazzaniga, M., ed., pp. 415–435. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (1995).
• Barone, P., Batardiere, A., Knoblauch, K., and Kennedy, H. “Laminar
distribution of neurons in extrastriate areas projecting to visual areas
V1 and V4 correlates with the hierarchical rank and indicates the
operation of a distance rule,” J. Neurosci. 20:3263–3281 (2000).
• Barrow, J.D., and Tipler, F.J. The Anthropic Cosmological Principle.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (1986).
• Bateson, W. “Review of The Mechanism of Mendelian Heredity by
T.H. Morgan, A.H. Sturtevant, H.J. Muller, and C.B. Bridges,” Science
44:536–543 (1916).
• Batista, A.P. and Andersen, R.A. “The parietal reach region codes the
next planned movement in a sequential reach task,” J. Neurophysiol.
85:539–544 (2001).
• Bauby, J.-D. The Diving Bell and the Butterfly: A Memoir of Life in
Death. New York: Alfred A. Knopf (1997).
• Bauer, R.M. and Demery, J.A. “Agnosia.” In: Clinical Neuropsychology.
4th ed., Heilman, K.M., and Valenstein, E., eds., pp. 236–295.
New York: Oxford University Press (2003).
• Bayne, T. and Chalmers, D.J. “What is the unity of consciousness?”
In: The Unity of Consciousness. Cleeremans, A., ed., pp. 23–58.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2003).
• Beckermann, A., Flohr, H., and Kim, J., eds. Emergence or Reduction?
Essays on the Prospects of Nonreductive Physicalism. Berlin:
Walter de Gruyter (1992).
参考文献6
• Beierlein, M., Gibson, J.R., and Connors, B.W. “A network of electrically
coupled interneurons drives synchronized inhibition in neocortex,”
Nature Neurosci. 3:904–910 (2000).
• Bennett, C.H. “Logical depth and physical complexity.” In: The Universal
Turing Machine. A Half-Century Survey. Herken, R., ed., pp.
227–258. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (1988).
• Benton, A. and Tranel, D. “Visuoperceptual, visuospatial, and visuoconstructive
disorders.” In: Clinical Neurosychology. 3rd ed., Heilman,
K.M. and Valenstein, E., eds., pp. 165–278. New York: Oxford
University Press (1993).
• Bergen, J.R. and Julesz, B. “Parallel versus serial processing in rapid
pattern discrimination,” Nature 303:696–698 (1983).
• Berns, G.S., Cohen, J.D., and Mintun, M.A. “Brain regions responsive
to novelty in the absence of awareness,” Science 276:1272–1275
(1997).
• Berti, A. and Rizzolatti, G. “Visual processing without awareness: Evidence
from unilateral neglect,” J. Cogn. Neurosci. 4:345–351 (1992).
• Bhalla, M. and Proffitt, D.R. “Visual-motor recalibration in geographical
slant perception,” J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Perception & Performance
25:1076–1096 (1999).
• Bialek W., Rieke, F., van Steveninck, R.R.D., andWarland, D. “Reading
a neural code,” Science 252:1854–1857 (1991).
• Biederman, I. “Perceiving real-world scenes,” Science 177:77–80 (1972).
• Billock, V.A. “Very short term visual memory via reverberation: A
role for the cortico-thalamic excitatory circuit in temporal filling-in
during blinks and saccades?” Vision Res. 37:949–953 (1997).
• Bisiach, E. and Luzzatti, C. “Unilateral neglect of representational
space,” Cortex 14:129–133 (1978).
• Bisley, J.W. and Goldberg, M.E. “Neuronal activity in the lateral
intraparietal area and spatial attention,” Science 299:81–86 (2003).
• Blackmore, S.J. Beyond the Body: An Investigation of Out-Of-The-
Body Experiences. London: Heinemann (1982).
参考文献7
• Blackmore, S., Brelstaff, G., Nelson, K., and Tsoscianko, T. “Is the
richness of our visual world an illusion? Transsaccadic memory for
complex scenes,” Perception 24:1075–1081 (1995).
• Blake, R. “A neural theory of binocular rivalry,” Psychol. Rev. 96:145–
167 (1989).
• Blake, R. “What can be “perceived” in the absence of visual awareness?”
Curr. Direction Psychol. Sci. 6:157–162 (1998).
• Blake, R. and Cormack, R.H. “On utrocular discrimination,” Perception
& Psychophysics 26:53–68 (1979).
• Blake, R. and Fox, R. “Adaptation to invisible gratings and the site
of binocular rivalry suppression,” Nature 249:488–490 (1974).
• Blake, R. and Logothetis, N.K. “Visual Competition,” Nature Rev.
Neurosci. 3:13–21 (2002).
• Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., and Seeck, M. “Stimulating illusory
own-body perceptions,” Nature 419:269–270 (2002).
• Blasdel, G.G. “Orientation selectivity, preference, and continuity in
monkey striate cortex,” J. Neurosci. 12:3139–3161 (1992).
• Blasdel, G.G. and Lund, J.S. “Termination of afferent axons in macaque
striate cortex,” J. Neurosci. 3:1389–1413 (1983).
• Blaser, E., Sperling, G., and Lu, Z.-L. “Measuring the amplification
of attention,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96:11681–11686 (1999).
• Blatow, M., Rozov, A., Katona, I., Hormuzdi, S.G., Meyer, A.H.,
Whittington, M.A., Caputi, A., and Monyer, H. “A novel network
of multipolar bursting interneurons generates theta frequency oscillations
in neocortex,” Neuron 38:805–817 (2003).
• Block, N. “On a confusion about a function of consciousness,” Behav.
Brain Sci. 18:227–247 (1995).
• Block, N. “How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness?”
Trends Neurosci. 19:456–459 (1996).
• Block, N., Flanagan, O., and G¨uzeldere, G., eds. Consciousness:
Philosophical Debates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1997).
• Bogen, J.E. “Mental duality in the intact brain,” Bull. Clinical Neurosci.
51:3–29 (1986).
参考文献8
• Bogen, J.E. “The callosal syndromes.” In: Clinical Neurosychology.
3rd ed., Heilman, K.M. and Valenstein, E., eds., pp. 337–407. New
York: Oxford University Press (1993).
• Bogen, J.E. “On the neurophysiology of consciousness: I. An overview,”
Consc. & Cognition 4:52–62 (1995a).
• Bogen, J.E. “On the neurophysiology of consciousness: II. Constraining
the semantic problem,” Consc. & Cognition 4:137–158 (1995b).
• Bogen, J.E. “Some neurophysiologic aspects of consciousness,” Sem.
Neurobiol. 17:95–103 (1997a).
• Bogen, J.E. “The neurosurgeon’s interest in the corpus callosum.” In:
A History of Neurosurgery in its Scientific and Professional Contexts.
Greenblatt S.H., ed., chapter 24. Park Ridge, IL: American Association
of Neurological Surgeons (1997b).
• Bogen, J.E. “Does cognition in the disconnected right hemisphere require
right hemisphere possession of language?” Brain & Language
57:12–21 (1997c).
• Bogen, J.E., Fisher, E.D., and Vogel, P.J. “Cerebral commissurotomy:
A second case report,” J. Am. Med. Assoc. 194:1328–1329 (1965).
• Bogen, J.E. and Gazzaniga, M.S. “Cerebral commissurotomy in man:
Minor hemisphere dominance for certain visuospatial functions,” J.
Neurosurg. 23:394–399 (1965).
• Bogen, J.E. and Gordon, H. W. “Musical tests for functional lateralization
with intracarotid amobarbital,” Nature 230:524–525 (1970).
• Bonneh, Y.S., Cooperman, A., and Sagi, D. “Motion-induced blindness
in normal observers,” Nature 411:798–801 (2001).
• Booth, M.C.A. and Rolls, E.T. “View-invariant representations of familiar
objects by neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex,” Cerebral
Cortex 8:510–523 (1998).
• Borrell, V. and Callaway, E.M. “Reorganization of exuberant axonal
arbors contributes to the development of laminar specificity in ferret
visual cortex,” J. Neurosci. 22:6682–6695 (2002).
• Bourassa, J. and Deschenes, M. “Corticothalamic projections from
the primary visual cortex in rats: A single fiber study using biocytin
as an anterograde tracer,” Neurosci. 66:253–263 (1995).
参考文献9
• Braak, H. “On the striate area of the human isocortex. A Golgi and
pigmentarchitectonic study,” J. Comp. Neurol. 166:341–364 (1976).
• Braak, H. Architectonics of the Human Telencephalic Cortex. Berlin:
Springer (1980).
• Bradley, D.C., Chang, G.C., and Andersen, R.A. “Encoding of threedimensional
structure-from-motion by primate areaMT neurons,” Nature
392:714–717 (1998).
• Braitenberg, V. and Sch¨uz, A. Anatomy of the Cortex. Heidelberg:
Springer (1991).
• Braun, J. “Visual search among items of different salience: Removal of
visual attention mimics a lesion in extrastriate area V4,” J. Neurosci.
14:554–567 (1994).
• Braun, J. “Natural scenes upset the visual applecart,” Trends Cogn.
Neurosci. 7:7–9 (2003).
• Braun, A.R., Balkin, T.J.,Wesensten, N.J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R.E.,
Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G., and Herscovitch, P. “Dissociated
pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human
rapid eye movement sleep,” Science 279:91–95 (1998).
• Braun, J. and Julesz, B. “Withdrawing attention at little or no cost:
Detection and discrimination tasks,” Perception & Psychophysics 60:1–
23 (1998).
• Braun, J., Koch, C., and Davis, J.L., eds. Visual Attention and Cortical
Circuits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2001).
• Braun, J. and Sagi, D. “Vision outside the focus of attention,” Perception
& Psychophysics 48:277–294 (1990).
• Brefczynski, J.A. and DeYoe, E.A. “A physiological correlate of the
‘spotlight’ of visual attention,” Nature Neurosci. 2:370–374 (1999).
• Breitmeyer, B.G. Visual Masking: An Integrative Approach. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press (1984).
• Breitmeyer, B.G. and ¨Ogmen, H. “Recent models and findings in backward
visual masking: A comparison, review and update,” Percept. &
Psychophysics 62:1572–1595 (2000).
参考文献10
• Brewer, A.A., Press, W.A., Logothetis, N.K., and Wandell, B.A. “Visual
areas in macaque cortex measured using functional magnetic resonance
imaging,” J. Neurosci. 22:10416–10426 (2002).
• Brickner, R.M. The Intellectual Functions of the Frontal Lobes. New
York: Macmillan (1936).
• Bridgeman, B., Hendry, D., and Stark, L. “Failure to detect displacement
of the visual world during saccadic eye movements,” Vision Res.
15:719–722 (1975).
• Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., and Sperling, A. “Segregation of cognitive
and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion,” Percept.
Psychophys. 29:336–342 (1981).
• Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., and Nagle, M. “Relation between
cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception,”
J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. 5:692–700 (1979).
• Bridgeman, B., Peery S., and Anand, S. “Interaction of cognitive
and sensorimotor maps of visual space,” Perception & Psychophysics
59:456–469 (1997).
• Brindley, G.S., Gautier-Smith, P.C., and Lewin, W. “Cortical blindness
and the functions of the non-geniculate fibres of the optic tracts,”
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 32:259–264 (1969).
• Britten, K.H., Newsome, W.T., Shadlen, M.N., Celebrini, S., and
Movshon, J.A. “A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual
responses of neurons in macaque MT,” Visual Neurosci. 13:87–
100 (1996).
• Britten, K.H., Shadlen, M.N., Newsome, W.T., and Movshon, A. “The
analysis of visual motion: A comparison of neuronal and psychophysical
performance,” J. Neurosci. 12:4745–4765 (1992).
• Broca, A. and Sulzer, D. “La sensation lumineuse fonction du temps,”
J. de Physiol. Taphol. Generale 4:632–640 (1902).
• Brodmann, K. “Physiologie des Gehirns,” Neue Deutsche Chirurgie
11:85–426 (1914).
• Brooke, R.N., Downes, J., and Powell, T.P. “Centrifugal fibres to the
retina in the monkey and cat,” Nature 207:1365–1367 (1965).
参考文献11
• Broughton, R., Billings, R., Cartwright, R., Doucette, D., Edmeads,
J., Edwardh, M., Ervin, F., Orchard, B., Hill, R., and Turrell, G.
“Homicidal somnambulism: A case report,” Sleep 17:253–264 (1994).
• Brown, E.N., Frank, L.M., Tang, D., Quirk, M.C., and Wilson, M.A.
“A statistical paradigm for neural spike train decoding applied to
position prediction from ensemble firing patterns of rat hippocampal
place cells,” J. Neurosci. 18:7411–7425 (1998).
• Brown, W.S., Murphy, N., and Malony, H.N., eds. Whatever Happened
to the Soul? Scientific and Theological Portraits of Human Nature.
Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press (1998).
• Bruce, C.J., Desimone, R., and Gross, C.G. “Both striate cortex and
superior colliculus contribute to visual properties of neurons in superior
temporal polysensory area of the macaque monkey,” J. Neurophysiol.
55:1057–1075 (1986).
• Budd, J.M. “Extrastriate feedback to primary visual cortex in primates:
A quantitative analysis of connectivity,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond.
B 265:1037–1044 (1998).
• Bullier, J. “Feedback connections and conscious vision,” Trends Cogn.
Sci. 5:369–370 (2001).
• Bullier, J., Girard, P., and Salin, P.-A. “The role of area 17 in the
transfer of information to extrastriate visual cortex.” In: Cerebral
Cortex Vol. 10. Peters, A. and Rockland, K.S., eds., pp. 301–330.
New York: Plenum Press (1994).
• Burkhalter, A. and Van Essen, D.C. “Processing of color, form and
disparity information in visual areas VP and V2 of ventral extrastiriate
cortex in the macaque monkey,” J. Neurosci. 6:2327–2351 (1986).
• Burle, B. and Bonnet, M. “Further argument for the existence of a
pacemaker in the human information processing system,” Acta Psychol.
97:129–143 (1997).
• Burle, B. and Bonnet, M. “What’s an internal clock for? From temporal
information processing to temporal processing of information,”
Behavioural Processes 45:59–72 (1999).
• Burr, D.C., Morrone, M.C., and Ross, R. “Selective suppression of
the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements,”
Nature 371:511–513 (1994).
参考文献12
• Buxhoeveden, D.P. and Casanova, M.F. “The minicolumn hypothesis
in neuroscience,” Brain 125:935–951 (2002).
• Buzs´aki, G. “Theta oscillations in the hippocampus,” Neuron 33:325–
340 (2002).
• Byrne, A. and Hilbert, D.R., eds. Readings on Color: The Science of
Color. Vol. 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1997).
• Calkins, D.J. “Representation of cone signals in the primate retina,”
J. Optical Soc. Am. A 17:597–606 (2000).
• Callaway, E.M. and Wiser, A.K. “Contributions of individual layer
2–5 spiny neurons to local circuits in macaque primary visual cortex,”
Vis. Neurosci. 13:907–922 (1996).
• Calvin, W.H. “Competing for consciousness: A Darwinian mechanism
of an appropriate level of explanation.” J. Consc. Studies 5:389–404
(1998).
• Calvin, W.H. and Ojemann, G.A. Conversations with Neil’s Brain.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley (1994).
• Campbell, K.K. Body and Mind. New York: Doubleday (1970).
• Carey, D.P. “Do action systems resist visual illusions?” Trends Cogn.
Sci. 5:109–113 (2001).
• Carmichael, S.T. and Price, J.L. “Architectonic subdivision of the orbital
and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey,” J. Comp.
Neurol. 346:366–402 (1994).
• Carrillo, M.C., Gabrieli, J.D.E., and Disterhoft, J.F. “Selective effects
of division of attention on discrimination conditioning,” PsychoBiol.
28:293–302 (2000).
• Carter, R.M., Hofst¨otter, C., Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. “Working
memory and fear conditoning,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
100:1399–1404 (2003).
• Castet, E. and Masson, G.S. “Motion perception during saccadic eye
movements,” Nature Neurosci. 3:177–183 (2000).
• Castiello, U., Paulignan, Y., and Jeannerod, M. “Temporal dissociation
of motor responses and subjective awareness,” Brain 114:2639–
2655 (1991).
参考文献13
• Cauller, L.J. and Kulics, A.T. “The neural basis of the behaviorally
relevant N1 component of the somatosensory-evoked potential in SI
cortex of awake monkeys: Evidence that backward cortical projections
signal conscious touch sensation,” Exp. Brain Res. 84:607–619
(1991).
• Cave, K.R. and Bichot, N.P. “Visuospatial attention: Beyond a spotlight
model,” Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 6:204–223 (1999).
• Celesia, G.G. “Persistent vegetative state: Clinical and ethical issues,”
Theor. Medicine 18:221–236 (1997).
• Celesia, G.G., Bushnell, D., Cone-Toleikis, S., and Brigell, M.G. “Cortical
blindness and residual vision: Is the second visual system in humans
capable of more than rudimentary visual perception?” Neurol.
41:862–869 (1991).
• Chalmers, D.J. The Conscious Mind: In Search of a Fundamental
Theory. New York: Oxford University Press (1996).(邦訳)林一訳,
意識する心:脳と精神の根本理論を求めて,白揚社(2001).
• Chalmers, D.J. “What is a neural correlate of consciousness?” In:
Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions.
Metzinger, T., ed., pp. 17–40. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(2000).
• Chalmers, D.J., ed. Philosophy of Mind: Classical and Contemporary
Readings. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2002).
• Changeux, J.P. L’homme neuronal. Paris: Fayard (1983).
• Chatterjee, S. and Callaway, E.M. “S cone contributions to the magnocellular
visual pathway in macaque monkey,” Neuron 35:1135–1146
(2002).
• Cheesman J. and Merikle, P.M. “Distinguishing conscious from unconscious
perceptual processes,” Can. J. Psychol. 40:3433–367 (1986).
• Chelazzi, L., Miller, E.K., Duncan, J., and Desimone, R. “A neural
basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex,” Nature 363:345–
347 (1993).
• Cherniak, C. “Neural component placement,” Trends Neurosci. 18:522–
527 (1995).
参考文献14
• Chun, M. M. and Wolfe, J. M. “Just say no: How are visual searches
terminated when there is no target present?” Cogn. Psychology
30:39–78 (1996).
• Churchland, P.S. Neurophilosophy. Cambridge,MA:MIT Press (1986).
(邦訳)信原幸弘,宮島昭二訳,認知哲学:脳科学から心の哲学へ,産
業図書(1997).
• Churchland, P.S. Brain-Wise: Studies in Neurophilosophy. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press (2002).
• Churchland, P.S. and Ramachandran, V.S. “Filling in: Why Dennett
is wrong.” In: Dennett and His Critics: Demystifying Mind.
Dahlbom, B., ed., pp. 28–52. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific (1993).
• Clark, R.E. and Squire, L.R. “Classical conditioning and brain systems:
The role of awareness,” Science 280:77–81 (1998).
• Clark, R.E. and Squire, L.R. “Human eyeblink classical conditioning:
Effects of manipulating awareness of the stimulus contingencies,”
Psychological Sci. 10:14–18 (1999).
• Cleeremans, A., et al. “Implicit learning: News from the front,”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 2:406–416 (1998).
• Cleeremans, A., ed. The Unity of Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press (2003).
• Clifford, C.W.G., Arnold, D.H., and Pearson, J. “A paradox of temporal
perception revealed by a stimulus oscillating in colour and orientation,”
Vision Res. 43:2245–2253 (2003).
• Colby, C.L. and Goldberg, M.E. “Space and attention in parietal cortex,”
Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 22:319–349 (1999).
• Cole, J. Pride and a Daily Marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1995).
• Coltheart, M. “Iconic memory,” Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B
302:283–294 (1983).
• Coltheart, V., ed. Fleeting Memories: Cognition of Brief Visual Stimuli.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1999).
• Colvin, M.K., Dunbar, K., and Grafman, J. “The effects of frontal lobe
lesions on goal achievement in the water jug task,” J. Cogn. Neurosci.
13:1139–1147 (2001).
参考文献15
• Compte, A., Brunel, N., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., andWang, X.J. “Synaptic
mechanisms and network dynamics underlying spatial working
memory in a cortical network model,” Cerebral Cortex 10:10–123
(2000).
• Conway, B.R., Hubel, D.H., and Livingstone, M.S. “Color contrast in
macaque V1,” Cerebral Cortex 12:915–925 (2002).
• Cook, E.P. and Maunsell, J.H.R. “Dynamics of neuronal responses in
macaque MT and VIP during motion detection,” Nature Neurosci.
5:985–994 (2002).
• Coppola, D. and Purves, D. “The extraordinary rapid disappearance
of entoptic images,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93:8001–8004
(1996).
• Corbetta, M. “Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention
and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping
neural systems?” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95:831–838 (1998).
• Corkin, S., Amaral, D.G., Gonzalez, R.G., Johnson, K.A., and Hyman,
B.T. “H. M.’s medial temporal lobe lesion: Findings from magnetic
resonance imaging,” J. Neurosci. 17:3964–3979 (1997).
• Cornell-Bell, A.H., Finkbeiner, S.M., Cooper, M.S., and Smith, S.J.
“Glutamate induces calcium waves in cultured astrocytes: Long-range
glial signaling,” Science 247:470–473 (1990).
• Cotterill, R. Enchanted Looms: Conscious Networks in Brains and
Computers. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press (1998).
• Courtney, S.M., Petit, L., Maisog, J.M., Ungerleider, L.G., and Haxby,
J.V. “An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal
cortex,” Science 279:1347–1351 (1998).
• Cowan, N. “The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration
of mental storage capacity,” Behav. Brain Sci. 24:87–185
(2001).
• Cowey, A. and Heywood, C.A. “Cerebral achromatopsia: Color blindness
despite wavelength processing,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 1:133–139
(1997).
• Cowey, A. and Stoerig, P. “The neurobiology of blindsight,” Trends
Neurosci. 14:140–145 (1991).
参考文献16
• Cowey, A. and Stoerig, P. “Blindsight in monkeys,” Nature 373:247–
249 (1995).
• Cowey, A. and Walsh, V. “Tickling the brain: Studying visual sensation,
perception and cognition by transcranial magnetic stimulation,”
Prog Brain Reserch 134:411–425 (2001).
• Creutzfeldt, O.D. Cortex Cerebri: Performance, Structural and Functional
Organization of the Cortex. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press (1995).
• Creutzfeldt, O.D. and Houchin, J. “Neuronal basis of EEG waves.”
In: Handbook of Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology.
Vol. 2., Remond, A., ed., pp. 3–55. Amsterdam, Netherlands:
Elsevier (1984).
• Crick, F.C. “Thinking about the brain,” Scientific American 241:219–
232 (1979).
• Crick, F.C. “Function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight
hypothesis,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 81:4586–4590 (1984).
• Crick, F.C. The Astonishing Hypothesis. New York: Charles Scribner’s
Sons (1994).(邦訳)中原英臣訳,DNA に魂はあるか:驚異の仮
説,講談社(1995).
• Crick, F.C. and Jones, E.G. “Backwardness of human neuroanatomy,”
Nature 361:109–110 (1993).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness,”
Sem. Neurosci. 2:263–275 (1990a).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Some reflections on visual awareness,” Cold
Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 55:953–962 (1990b).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “The problem of consciousness,” Sci. Am.
267:153–159 (1992).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Are we aware of neural activity in primary
visual cortex?” Nature 375:121–123 (1995a).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Why neuroscience may be able to explain
consciousness,” Sci. Am. 273:84–85 (1995b).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Constraints on cortical and thalamic projections:
The no-strong-loops hypothesis,” Nature 391:245–250 (1998a).
参考文献17
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “Consciousness and neuroscience,” Cerebral
Cortex 8:97–107 (1998b).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “The Unconscious Homunculus. With commentaries
by multiple authors,” Neuro-Psychoanalysis 2:3–59 (2000).
• Crick, F.C. and Koch, C. “A framework for consciousness,” Nature
Neurosci. 6:119–126 (2003).
• Crunelli, V. and Leresche, N. “Childhood absence epilepsy: Genes,
channels, neurons and networks,” Nature Rev. Neurosci. 3:371–382
(2002).
• Culham, J.C., Brandt, S.A., Cavanagh, P., Kanwisher, N.G., Dale,
A.M., and Tootell, R.B. “Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive
tracking of moving targets,” J. Neurophysiol. 80:2657–2670
(1998).
• Cumming, B.G. and DeAngelis, G.C. “The physiology of stereopsis,”
Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 24:203–238 (2001).
• Cumming, B.G. and Parker, A.J. “Responses of primary visual cortical
neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception,” Nature
389:280–283 (1997).
• Cumming, B.G. and Parker, A.J. “Binocular neurons in V1 of awake
monkeys are selective for absolute, not relative, disparity,” J. Neurosci.
19:5602–5618 (1999).
• Cumming, B.G. and Parker, A.J. “Local disparity not perceived depth
is signalled by binocular neurons in cortical area V1 of the macaque,”
J. Neurosci. 20:4758–4767 (2000).
• Curcio, C.A., Allen, K.A., Sloan, K.R., Lerea, C.L. Hurley, J.B.,
Klock, I.B., and Milam, A.H. “Distribution and morphology of human
cone photoreceptors stained with anti-blue opsin,” J. Comp. Neurol.
312:610–624 (1991).
• Curran, T. “Implicit learning revealed by the method of opposition,”
Trends Cogn. Sci. 5:503–504 (2001).
• Cytowic, R.E. The Man Who Tasted Shapes. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press (1993).
• Dacey, D.M. “Circuitry for color coding in the primate retina,” Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93:582–588 (1996).
参考文献18
• Dacey, D.M., Peterson, B.B., Robinson, F.R., and Gamlin, P.D. “Fireworks
in the primate retina: In vitro photodynamics reveals diverse
LGN-projecting ganglion cell types,” Neuron 37:15–27 (2003).
• Damasio, A.R. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in
the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace (1999).(邦
訳)田中三彦訳,無意識の脳 自己意識の脳:身体と情動と感情の神
秘,講談社(2003).
• Damasio, A.R. “A neurobiology for consciousness.” In: Neural Correlates
of Consciousness: Empirical and Conceptual Questions. Metzinger,
T., ed., pp. 111–120. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000).
• Damasio, A.R. and Anderson, S.W. “The frontal lobes.” In: Clinical
Neuropsychology. 4th ed., Heilman, K.M. and Valenstein, E. eds., pp.
404–446. New York: Oxford University Press (2003).
• Damasio, A.R., Eslinger, P., Damasio, H., Van Hoesen, G.W., and
Cornell, S. “Multimodal amnesic syndrome following bilateral temporal
and basal forebrain damage,” Arch. Neurol. 42:252–259 (1985).
• Damasio, A.R., Tranel, D., and Rizzo, M. “Disorders of complex visual
processing.” In: Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.
Mesulam, M.M., ed., pp. 332–372. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press (2000).
• Damasio, A.R., Yamada, T., Damasio, H., Corbet, J., and McKee,
J. “Central achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic and physiologic aspects,”
Neurol. 30:1064–1071 (1980).
• Dantzker, J.L. and Callaway, E.M. “Laminar sources of synaptic input
to cortical inhibitory interneurons and pyramidal neurons,” Nature
Neurosci. 7:701–707 (2000).
• Das, A. and Gilbert, C.D. “Distortions of visuotopic map match orientation
singularities in primary visual cortex,” Nature 387:594–598
(1997).
• Davis, W. Passage of Darkness: The Ethnobiology of the Haitian Zombie.
Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press (1988).
• Dawson M.E. and Furedy, J.J. “The role of awareness in human differential
autonomic classical conditioning: The necessary gate hypothesis,”
Psychophysiology 13:50–53 (1976).
参考文献19
• Dayan P. and Abbott, L. Theoretical Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press (2001).
• DeAngelis, G.C., Cumming, B.G., and Newsome, W.T. “Cortical area
MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth,” Nature 394:677–680
(1998).
• DeAngelis, G.C. and Newsome, W.T. “Organization of disparity-selective
neurons in macaque area MT,” J. Neurosci. 19:1398–1415 (1999).
• de Fockert, J.W., Rees, G., Frith, C.D., and Lavie, N. “The role of
working memory in visual selective attention,” Science 291:1803–1806
(2001).
• Dehaene, S. “Temporal oscillations in human perception,” Psychol.
Sci. 4:264–270 (1993).
• Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J.-P. “Neural mechanisms for access to
consciousness.” In: The Cognitive Neurosciences. 3rd ed., Gazzaniga,
M., ed., in press. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2004).
• Dehaene, S. and Naccache, L. “Towards a cognitive neuroscience of
consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework,” Cognition
79:1–37 (2001).
• Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin J.-F., Poline
J.-B., and Riv`ere, D. “Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and
unconscious repetition priming,” Nature Neurosci. 4:752–758 (2001).
• Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., and Changeux, J.P. “A neuronal model
linking subjective report and objective neurophysiological data during
conscious perception,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100:8520–8525
(2003).
• de Lima, A.D., Voigt, T., and Morrison, J.H. “Morphology of the cells
within the inferior temporal gyrus that project to the prefrontal cortex
in the macaque monkey,” J. Comp. Neurol. 296:159–172 (1990).
• Dennett, D. Content and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
(1969).
• Dennett, D. Brainstorms. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1978).
• Dennett, D. Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little & Brown (1991).
(邦訳)山口泰司訳,解明される意識,青土社(1998).



共通テーマ:健康

うつ病の症状改善プロセス



共通テーマ:健康

強迫スペクトラム障害(OCSD)



共通テーマ:健康

ナルシス・ナル君

まず芸能関係から引用
*****
矢田亜希子が赤ちゃんを産んだっていうニュースが報じられたけど、そのほとんどが、ダンナの押尾学が、自分たちのことを「美男美女の夫婦」って言ってのけたって部分をピックアップして伝えてた。たとえば、「日刊スポーツ」には、こんなふうに書いてあった。

(前略)押尾は「男でも女でもどっちでもいい。健康であればね。でもさ、美男美女の夫婦の子供で、かわいい子が生まれたの見たことがない」と、語っていた。(後略)

それで、スポーツ紙の記事を見ながら放送してたワイドショーでも、この部分を取り上げて、「自分で自分たちのことを美男美女だなんて、押尾さんらしいですね」ってコメントして、笑いのネタにしてた。だから、きっと、スポーツ紙を読んだ人たちも、ワイドショーを見た人たちも、みんな、おんなじように思っただろう。なんせ、押尾学は、「押尾語録」ってのが作られてるほど、自画自賛の嵐の天然男だから、こうした記事を読めば、誰もが「いかにも押尾学が言いそうなセリフだ」って思うからだ。

この流れを知れば、押尾学がお得意の自画自賛を炸裂させたんじゃなくて、何でもない普通のやり取りだったってことが分かったと思う。そして、このセリフは、「またまた押尾学が自画自賛のアホ発言をした」って思わせるために、悪意を持って編集されたものだってことも分かったと思う。

押尾学は、「赤ちゃんが男の子だった」ってことに対して、「男でも女でもどっちでもいい。健康であればね」って答えたワケで、そのあとの「美男美女のカップルですから、きっとかわいいお子さんなんでしょうね?」って質問に対して、「でもさ、美男美女の夫婦の子供で、かわいい子が生まれたの見たことがない」って答えたワケだ。だから、このやり取りには、どこにも、押尾チックな部分、テングになってる部分はない。それなのに、この2つのセリフを1つのカギカッコでくくって、いかにも続けてしゃべったように編集されたことによって、ミゴトなまでの押尾ワールドが全開になったってワケだ。

-----
というわけで、押尾学という人は、銀色夏生だと「ナルシス・ナル君」で、ある先生の好きな言い方だと「ナルちゃん」で、正式の言い方では、ナルシスティック・パーソナリティである、ということらしい。
これが病気にまで高まると、Narcissistic Personality Disorder と呼ばれ、発音はナルシシスティックであるが、省略して、ナルシスティックでもよいと、小此木先生がおっしゃった。日本語で、自己愛性人格障害である。ナーシスティック。
上記の中に、自画自賛とかテングとかの言葉が見えている。

-----
「あらゆる星が北極星を中心として動いているように、世界は私を中心として動いている。私は秩序そのものであり、法律そのものである」
シェークスピア「ジュリアス・シーザー」

シーザーだから、これでいい。ただの人がこんな風に思っているとして、ひそかに思って、日記に綴っているだけなら、害はない。他人の前で露出してしまうから、問題で、嫌われる。しかし、きらいな人は離れてゆくだけで、おおむねは問題ないのだが、上司と部下だったり、会社の同僚だったりすると、苦手な人だから離れているというわけにもいかない。上司がシーザーだったら、どうします?
ひょっとして、「そんな人こそ私にふさわしい、喜んでついていく」という人もいるだろうが、あなたもちょっとナルちゃんだ。

-----
「こんなあさましい身と成り果てた今でも、己は、己の詩集が長安風流人士の机の上に置かれている様を、夢に見ることがあるのだ。岩窟の中に横たわって見る夢にだよ。嗤ってくれ。詩人になりそこなって虎になった哀れな男を」  ―― 中島敦 「山月記」より

これもまあ、妻子には迷惑をかけたが、こんな人もいなければ、世の中は進歩しないので、これでいい。わたしはこの人をナルシスティックとはあまり思わないけれど。

自己愛性人格障害は、もっと平凡な人生を歩いていて、平凡な人間なのに、なぜか自分だけ特別だと思っているのである。理由がないのに。
自己愛性人格障害(Narcissistic Personality Disorder)とは、ありのままの自分を愛せず、自分は優越的で素晴らしく特別で偉大な存在でなければならないと思い込む人格障害である。

押尾学と矢田亜希子ならば、かなり特別なのであって、ナルちゃんとは私は思わない。

問題なのは、たとえば、こんな人たち。

御都合主義的な白昼夢に耽る。
自分のことにしか関心がない。
高慢で横柄な態度。
特別な人間であると思っている。
自分は特別な人間にしか理解されないと思っている。
冷淡で、他人を利用しようとする。
批判に対して過剰に反応する。
虚栄心から、嘘をつきやすい。
有名人の追っかけ。
宗教の熱烈な信者。

こんな感じのことを文章で描写すると、次のようになる。

なんでも自分の思い通りになるという空想に耽る。万能感の空想。すべて自分にとって都合のいいように事が運んで、最後には自分が絶大な称賛を浴びるという空想。自分だけが特別に評価されて大抜擢され、とんとん拍子に出世するという空想。
聞かれもしないのに、やたらと自分のことをしゃべりたがる人。話が他へ移ろうとすると、強引に自分の話に戻そうとする。話の内容は自慢話ばかりで、聞いている方はうんざりする。他人にはあまり関心がないので、相手がうんざりしていようとお構いなし。
自分は特別な人間だと確信している。小市民的な生き方を軽蔑し、そういう人達と一緒にされることを嫌う。裏付けとなるものがなにもないのに、一目置かれる存在であることに非常にこだわる。
あるいは、自分という人間は特別な人しか理解することができないのだと思ったりする。たとえば、以前マスターソンがラジオで自己愛人格障害の話をしたところ、自分は自己愛人格障害なのでぜひ治療してもらいたいという人が何人も電話してきた。そこでそのうちの十人を治療することになったが、実際に治療するのは高名なマスターソン本人ではないと知ったとき、十人が十人とも治療を断った。無名の医師ではダメ。
他人に対する共感に乏しく、他人を自分のために利用する。他人の業績を横取りして自分のものにする。優越感に浸るために他人を利用する。
もともと、裏付けのない優越感だから、話のつじつまを合わせるために嘘をつくこともある。本人には嘘をついているという意識はあまりない。ときにはホラ話のように、話がどんどん大きくなっていって、どこまで本当なのか分からなくなる。
有名人に近付くことで自分を特別な存在だと思い込んだりする。政治的な大物に近付いて自分の誇大感を膨らませることもある。自分も同じ世界の人間になったように錯覚して、裏付けのない空想的な野心にのめり込んだりすることもある。

誇大感を持つ人には二つのタイプがある。自分は素晴らしいと言うタイプと、あなたは素晴らしいというタイプである。あなたは素晴らしいというタイプの人は、その素晴らしい人に奉仕している私も素晴らしい特別な存在だと信じる。偉大な独裁者を崇拝する献身的な国民、偉大な神に身を捧げる熱狂的な信者、ワンマン経営者に心酔して滅私奉公する素晴らしい幹部社員、有名な歌手の応援をする熱狂的なファンなど。

すべてに言えることは、ありのままの自分が愛せないこと。自分は優越的な存在でなければならない。素晴らしい特別な存在であり、偉大な輝きに満ちた存在でなければならない。愛すべき自分は、とにかく輝いていなければならない。
しかし、これはありのままの自分ではないので、現実的な裏付けを欠くことになる。無理を通すので、時々は、現実にそぐわないことになる。

しかし、本人にしてみれば、高慢だと言われてもぴんと来ない。それよりは、その人に対して、「あなたは、他人や周囲の出来事を過小評価している」と言った方が理解されやすいかもしれない。自分より優れたものを認めたがらず馬鹿にしているので、他人の能力や才能が見えず、他人の優秀さを無視する。そして、他人を見下したり軽蔑したりすることに快感を覚える。

こんなタイプが上司だったら、どうします?

-----
ナルシシズムに特徴的な信念体系と性格行動パターンを抽出してみる。通していえることは、「自分は特別だ」という信念を抱き続けるために、無理をする、現実を無視するということだ。

1.私は、普通の大衆とは異なる特別に非凡で優れた人間である。……おおむね、一般大衆が気に入らない。自分は特別なのだから、一般大衆は、愚かでなければならない。
2.私は、目立ちたがり屋でいつも皆に注目されていたい。……自分より目立つ人がいてはならない。
3.私は、自分の容姿や知性、能力、所作、実績に自信を持っている。……自分は特別なのだから自信を持たなければならない。
4.私は、他人の意見に押し流されないだけの強い自分の意見を持ち、それを理路整然と主張することができる。……自分は特別なのだから、意見も主張も、特別な価値がなければならない。
5.私は、今以上の尊敬や賞賛、崇拝を受けて当然の存在である。……自分は特別なのだから、現在は不遇でも、将来、賞賛を集めなければならない。
6.私は、同一人物に対する評価が極端に変化して気分の波が激しい。(自分を低く評価したり、自分の意見に反対する相手を、公正に評価することはできないし許すことができない)……自分は特別なのだから、そのことを認めない他人に対しては、攻撃しなければならない。逆に、自分を賞讃するなら、その人も特別な人間に属する。
7.私は、鏡に向かって自分の姿を眺めるのが好きで落ち着く。……自分は特別なのだから、まず自分が、自分のすばらしさを確認しなければならない。
8.私の周りには、私を慕って尊敬する多くの人間(取り巻き)がいるべきだと思う。……自分は特別なのだから、周囲の人間は自分を尊敬しなければならない。

以上のように、おおむね、「自分は特別である」という信念を保持し強化するための行動であると解釈できる。

-----
いつも出てくる、DSM-IVをみてみると、こんな風。
自己愛性人格障害

誇大性(空想または行動における)、賞賛されたいという欲求、共感の欠如の広範な様式で、成人期早期に始まり、種々の状況で明らかになる。
以下のうち5つ(またはそれ以上)で示される。

自己の重要性に関する誇大な感覚(例:業績や才能を誇張する、十分な業績がないにもかかわらず優れていると認められることを期待する)。

限りない成功、権力、才気、美しき、あるいは理想的な愛の空想にとらわれている。

自分が特別であり、独特であり、他の特別なまたは地位の高い人達に(または施設で)しか理解されない、または関係があるべきだ、と信じている。

過剰な賞賛を求める。

特権意識つまり、特別有利な取り計らい、または自分の期待に自動的に従うことを理由なく期待する。

対人関係で相手を不当に利用する、つまり、自分自身の目的を達成するために他人を利用する。

共感の欠如:他人の気持ちおよび欲求を認識しようとしない、またはそれに気づこうとしない。

しばしば他人に嫉妬する、または他人が自分に嫉妬していると思い込む。

尊大で傲慢な行動 または態度。

-----
つねにこんな感じという人も多くはないけれど、一時的に、こんな感じが強くなるといった程度の人なら、少なくない。

程度の問題だし、場面の問題でもある。ナル君になっていい場面でも、あくまでも謙虚と言うのも、困りものだ。時と場合に応じて、適応的な行動が取れるのが大人である。たとえば、恋愛の一場面では、ナル君になってくれないと、話が続かない。

「自分は特別である」という確信は、残念ながら、現実に反する確信なので、(つまり、妄想的なので)、現実と矛盾し、折り合いがつかず、他人を困惑させ、不愉快にさせてしまう場面も出てくる。
普通は、そのような場面での気まずさから、これではいけないと気付き、現実的な路線をとるものだ。
しかしナルシス・ナル君たちは、あくまで、「自分は特別だ」と信じ続けたいのだ。そのことが現実的な利益をもたらすわけではないし、友人もいなくなり、自分は抑うつ的になり、いいことばかりではないのだが、それでも、「自分は特別だ」と信じたいのだ。

-----
自己愛性人格障害(ナルシスト)とは、「自分自身を愛する」という行為が病的なほど大きくなり、「自分は重要な人間だ」「周りには自分を理解できない」といった誇大感を持つようになり、ありのままの自分を愛することができず、空想的誇大的自己を保持するために不自然な事態となっている人である。

自分を愛するという行為は、健全な心の発達のためには必要なものであるが、程度がすぎると問題である。
ありのままの自分を愛することができるなら、健全な人である。
しかし、ありのままの自分に満足できないというのも、向上心と考えれば、一面では、悪くない面がある。一時的で限定的なら、いいことなのである。

惨めな現実を受け入れられず、空想的な誇大的な自己を作り出している。これも、自分を守るための、一時的なことならば、いいことでもある。

惨めな自分をすっきりと受け入れて、気持ちも惨めにならないということは難しい。適当なところで、多少の脚色をしながら、自分を慰めながら、だんだんと現実の惨めさを受け入れてゆくものだ。ナル君たちは、その、脚色から、いつまでたっても、抜けられない人と言ってもいいだろう。

でも、そういう人たちには、一種特別なエネルギーがあることがある。「自分は特別」なのだから、人一倍努力する部分があったり、リスクを恐れずゲームに参加したり、そんな面もある。そして、成功してしまえば、その人は一気に、現実に、賞賛にふさわしい人になる。だから、若いうちの自己愛性傾向は、悪いことでもない。いつまでもそのままというのは人生を難しくしてしまうけれど。

「俺は特別だ、甲子園に行くぞ。親父はそのために金を稼いでくれ。お袋は栄養管理をしてくれ。」というくらいでないと、なかなか甲子園にはいけない。「わたしは特別だ、もっときれいになって、せりふも覚えて、デビューするぞ」というくらいでないと、芸能界デビューも難しい。

自分の息子をプロ野球選手にしようとして特別に育てたという人の一部には、ナル君が混じっているのかもしれない。誰でも、自分の子供の将来については、過大な期待を抱き、夢を見る。それは社会的に容認された、ひそかなナルシスティック傾向なのかもしれない。

ーーーーー
誇大感に満ちた空想は現実感を奪う。たとえば、本当は自分が他人に嫉妬しているのに、他人が自分に嫉妬していると思ったりする。他人から批判されると、あれは私に嫉妬しているからだと解釈する。こういう防衛機制によって現実を再解釈して、納得しようとする。

ナルシス・ナル君はなにがなんでも自分が優位に立つ必要がある。どんな卑怯な手段を使ってでも、どんなにつじつまの合わない妄想であろうと、自分を守るためにしがみつかざるを得ない。嘘をついたり、他人を利用したりすることも、「自分は特別だ」という信念を補強するためと考えれば、合目的的なのである。

他人から侮辱されたと思い込んだりした場合、自分を守るために、非常に激しく怒ったりする。あまりにも自己中心的な怒り方であり、周囲の反感を買うが、本人は必死である。自己愛的憤怒と呼んでいる。

妄想の種になるようなものがないときは、他人の欠点を捜し出して見下したりする。ありとあらゆる理由をつけて他人を見下す。貧乏人の癖に、不細工な顔をしているくせに、頭が悪いくせに。実際にどうであるかということよりも、とにかく見下すことができればそれでいい。それは、ただ、「自分は特別だ」という信念を補強したいからだ。

他人を見下すということは、ときには他人からの報復攻撃として、自分が陥れられるかもしれないという疑いを生み、非常に疑い深くなったりする。他人に心を開くことなく、自分の妄想の殻の中に閉じこもる。これは二次的な必然である。大抵は、こんなことになったらつらいので、「自分は特別だ」信念を捨てるのだが、この人たちは、捨てない。

こういったことは様々な不都合を生む。しかし自我の崩壊を防ぐことができる。もし、妄想が崩れたら一気にうつ状態になったり、あるいはパニックになったりする。少なくとも妄想にしがみついていられる間はこのような悲惨な状態にはならない。

WHOのICD-10では正式な精神障害としては採用されていない。

境界性人格障害でも原因として日本では過保護、アメリカでは虐待が多いという指摘があるが、自己愛性人格障害に関しても似たような言説がある。私としては、そのまま信じることはできない。

まずフロイト、マスターソン、コフートを勉強する。日本語なら、小此木またはその一派の文献が基本。

自己愛(narcissism, self-love)ヘイベロック・エリス(Havelock Ellis)
フロイト(S.Freud, 1856-1939)性的精神発達理論(リビドー発達論)『乳幼児期の正常な自己愛』と『思春期以降の異常な自己愛』

S.フロイトの一次性ナルシシズムと二次性ナルシシズム
一次性ナルシシズムは、『母子分離不安』を弱めようとする防衛機制
マーガレット・S・マーラー(1897-1985)『分離・個体化期(separation-individuation)』

S.フロイトは、正常なリビドー(性的欲動)の充足対象の変遷として『自体愛→自己愛→対象愛の発達ライン』を考えていたので、異性に性的な関心が芽生えてくる思春期以降の二次性ナルシシズムは『病的な性倒錯』であると主張した。思春期や成人期にある男女が、自分の持つ魅力(属性)に自己愛的に陶酔したり、自己の性的身体を自体愛的に欲望するのは、リビドー発達が障害された結果としての性倒錯であり、成人の持つべき生殖能力を失わせる『幼児的な部分性欲への退行』であると言う。

フロイト『自己愛から対象愛への移行』『本能変遷』

二次性ナルシシズム……退行(regression)と固着(fixation)

コフートの自己心理学と自己愛理論。
フロイトは、自己愛(ナルシシズム)を病理的な性倒錯の一種であると考えた。
しかし、自己心理学のハインツ・コフート(Heinz Kohut, 1913)は、自己肯定的な自尊心と活動性の原動力となる『自己愛の発達』を正常な精神発達ラインの一つとした。
『健全な自己愛(self-love)』の成長を促進することで、他者を共感的に思いやる対象愛(object love)も発達し、社会活動に積極的に参加しようとする適応性も高まる。

コフートの自己愛とは、『自己存在の積極的な肯定』であり『理想化と関係する自尊心(自信)の基盤』である。
コフートの自己心理学には『自己愛と対象愛は表裏一体である(自己愛がなければ他者を愛せない)』という信念がある。フロイトにとって、自己愛は『自己愛から対象愛への正常な移行(本能変遷)』を成し遂げられなかった病理的状態であるが、コフートにとっての自己愛は『誰もが持つべき自己肯定感(自尊心)の基盤』であり、苛酷な人生を乗り切る為に必要な“生のエネルギー”の源泉である。

コフートは自己愛の定義として、『自分自身を愛する自己愛』と『自己対象を愛する自己愛』を挙げている。つまり、コフートの言う自己愛とは、自分にとって大切な他者である“自己対象(self-object)”を含むものであり、単純に、内向的かつ排他的な自己愛ではない。(自己心理学では、対象 object という言葉は独自の意味を持っている。)

意欲的な創造性や適度な自尊心を生み出す『健全な自己愛』に対して、自己愛性人格障害の原因となる『病的な自己愛』というのは、自分自身の権力や幸福、名声、成功のみに固執し、自己顕示欲求と誇大妄想的な自己陶酔を満たす為に、他人を不当に攻撃したり身勝手に利用したりする“過剰な自己愛”である。過剰な自己愛は、『尊大さ・傲慢さ・横柄さ』といった言葉で表現される、他者を侮蔑して否定する行動(発言・態度)となって現れる。

実際の自分以上に自分に価値があると妄想的に思い込み、『自分は凡人とは違う特別な人間だから、もっと丁重に敬意を持って扱われるべきだ』といった要求を明示的・暗示的に主張し、『私の実力や魅力、価値を評価できない人間は、物事の価値が分からない無能な人間であり付き合う価値がない(私の実力を高く評価できる人間は、私には及ばないもののなかなか優秀な人間である)』といった排他的かつ独善的な態度を示すこともある。他者に自分への賞賛と従属、関心を強制して、自分の力を認めない人や自分に従わない人たちを遠ざけることで(お世辞やご追従を言うご機嫌取りを周囲に集めることで)、外部の現実原則から自分を守ろうとする。

こんな人がいてもかまわないけれど、そして、その人がそれで得をするもと思わないけれど、そんな人と否応なく付き合わなければならない人は大変だ。やっと異動になって自分は逃れられたと思ったら、友人が今度はその人の部下になってしまい、慰め役になってあげたり。

自己愛が過剰に強くなることで、自己愛性人格障害や演技性人格障害といった病理的な人格構造が形成されてくると、現実的な自己評価を逸脱する誇大自己の拡大が起こり、その結果として特異的な行動パターンを示す。即ち、尊大(横柄)な態度や傲慢な発言が多くなり、自己顕示欲(エゴイズム)を満たす為に他人を利用しようとする。本人は『自分には他人を利用して満足を得る当然の権利と能力がある』と思い込んでいるので反省しない。自分を批判する者や自分の価値を引き下げる対応をする者は許すことが出来ないので、衝動的に激しく攻撃したり(自己愛性憤怒)、防衛的に無視して距離を取ろうとする。病的な自己愛の持ち主と一緒に居る相手は、独特な不快感や違和感を味わわされることになり、自己愛性人格障害の人は、一般的にわがままで自己顕示欲が強い人、傲慢不遜で非常識な性格の持ち主といった形で認知される。

『病的な自己愛=自己愛性人格障害の原因となる自己愛』もあるものの、自己心理学の自己愛理論では、適切に自己愛の強度と内容を調整できるのであれば、自己愛性人格障害のような人格構造の歪曲の問題は起きないと考える。
『良い自己評価を伴う正常な心理構造』の一部である自己愛や承認欲求(社会的欲求)は誰もが持っているものであり、自己愛そのものが病理的な悪影響をもたらすのではなく、自己愛のバランスの崩れや調節障害が自己愛性人格障害の苦悩や被害を生み出す。このあたりの論述がフロイトとの相違点である。

コフートは、人間は誰もが発達早期に受けた心理的な傷つき(欲求充足の欠如)を抱えているという『欠損モデル』を前提にしている。
生まれて間もない自他未分離の状態にある乳幼児は、母親・父親からの保護と世話を必要としており、その生存を全面的に養育者に依拠している。発達早期(0~2歳くらい)の乳幼児と母親は、二人の間にある境界線を意識しておらず、『幻想的な母子一体感』に浸った状態にある。

マーラー、ウィニコット、ボールビーなど。母子関係を扱ったりしているものは、なかなか難解。

しかし、実際には母親も不完全な人間で、子どもにミルクを与える時間が遅れたり、離れた場所にいて子どもの泣き声が聴こえなかったり、子どもが我がままを言って母親が怒ったりすることがある。そういった瞬間に幻想的な母子一体感が破られて、乳児は『幼児的な全能感』が通用しない欲求不満を感じ、『欠損モデル(defect model)』でいう心(自己愛)の傷つきや欠損を体験する。こういった欠損の存在は全ての人間にあるものであり、発達早期に欠損を感じた経験が自己の不完全さや欲求不満につながり、理想化や誇大性を求める『自己愛の起源』になる。

H.コフートは、自己愛の発達を対象愛の発達と同様に『正常な精神発達過程の一つ』と考えた。
中核自己(nuclear self, 私が私であるという自意識)の自己愛の発達過程には、『誇大自己(grandiose self)』と『理想化された親イマーゴ(idealized parent imago)』という二つのラインがある。欠損モデルに基づく中核自己(自我意識)は、向上心と理想という二つの極(方向性)を持つので双極自己(bipolar self)とも呼ばれる。
誇大自己(grandiose self)は「向上心」の極で発達していき、理想化された親イマーゴ(idealized parent imago)は「理想」の極で発達していく。

共感的な親(反応性の良い温かい母親)の元で、「誇大自己(grandiose self)」の自己愛の発達に成功すると、現実原則に適応できる成熟した誇大自己(自尊心や向上心の基盤)が成長し、非共感的な親(反応性の乏しい冷たい母親)の元で誇大自己ラインの自己愛の発達に失敗すると、幼稚な快楽原則に支配された未成熟な誇大自己(自己顕示欲の強い傲慢さや横柄さ)が強くなる。発達早期の母子関係を重視したコフートは、非共感的な親が乳児の心的構造の欠損(心的な外傷)を大きくして、乳児の精神内界に自己表象の「断片化(fragmentation)」を引き起こし、自己愛の病理の発症リスクを高めると考えた。

自己表象の「断片化(fragmentation)」なんていうことが乳児の精神内界で起こるのかな。でもまあ、このあたり、コーハット的。kohutをコフートとは発音しないだろうな。

「理想化された親イマーゴ(idealized parent imago)」では、親という表象(イマーゴ)や自己対象を理想化して同一化しようとする。その為、発達早期の親が非共感的な反応を示して乳児を無視したり拒絶したりすると、乳児は「最適な欲求不満(optimal frustration)」を経験することが出来なくなり、自己対象である親を理想化する契機(チャンス)を失う。「理想化された親イマーゴ」の形成に失敗するということは、心理内面に安定的に存在して自己愛の支えとなる「対象恒常性」の確立に失敗するということと同義であり、外界に対応する為の心理構造が非常に不安定になる。

共感的な親の元で、「理想化された親イマーゴ」の自己愛の発達に成功すると、自己愛性人格障害の原因となる『不適応な誇大自己』の発達を抑制することが出来るが、それは、精神内界に安定した「自己対象の恒常性」が確立することで「過剰防衛を行う誇大な自己」を強調する必要性がなくなるからである。子どもは自分に価値があるという実感や自分が評価されているという満足の原初的体験を親子関係の中でしていくが、そういった共感的な被承認体験が出来ないと、自己愛的な賞賛と評価を必死に求める誇大自己の拡大が見られるようになる。子どもに対して完全に無関心だったり拒絶的だったりする“冷たい母親”の元では、「理想化された親イマーゴ」の形成に必然的に失敗するので、理想化の自己対象を見失って衝動的な行動や抑うつ的な反応が目立ってくることになる。親の子どもに対する徹底的な無視や冷たい対応というのは、精神分析的な心因論では、うつ病や統合失調症、ボーダーライン(境界例)、境界性人格障害、自己愛性人格障害、反社会性人格障害などの原因になると考えられている。実証的な統計学的研究(疫学的研究)のエビデンスが十分に積み重ねられていないので、「愛情不足の親子関係」だけがそれらの精神疾患(人格障害)の危険因子になるわけではない。

少し前までは、発達早期の心的外傷のようなことがよく言われたが、最近は、やや下火であるように思う。

向上心を伴う『誇大自己(grandiose self)』と理想を構築する『理想化された親イマーゴ(idealized parent imago)』が相互作用することで進む心的構造の形成過程を『変容性内在化(transmuting internalization)』という。コフートの自己心理学に基づくと、自己愛性人格障害の人格形成過程とは、変容性内在化(transmuting internalization)の不適切な進行であり、もっと正確に言うならば、自己成熟へと向かう『誇大自己』と『理想化された親イマーゴ』のバランスの取れた統合的発展の失敗であると言うことが出来る。

誇大自己は、自分自身の鏡像を自己対象とする『鏡面化』の発達過程をたどる最も純粋な自己愛のルーツ(起源)であるが、乳幼児は鏡に写った自分の鏡像を見てナルシシスティックに自己の強力な力や有能性を確信する。露出的で誇大妄想的な誇大自己が強くなりすぎると、自己顕示や支配的野心の抑制を欠いて自己愛性人格障害の原因となる。理想化された親イマーゴは、自分の両親(養育者)を自己対象とする『理想化』の発達過程を形成し、発達早期の母子関係に問題があると、「過剰な誇大自己の発達水準」に固着が起こる。「過剰な誇大自己の発達水準」へと防衛的に退行することで、誇大な自己顕示性と利己主義を特徴とする自己愛性人格障害の行動パターンが生まれる。

「鏡」の比喩はいろいろと活躍する。私も大好きな比喩だ。鏡の不思議さと、自己が自己を認知する不思議さは、やはり、重なり合うはずだと思う。

自己愛性人格障害に見られる傲慢不遜な態度や過度の自己顕示欲を精神分析的に分析すると、『正常な自己イメージと対象恒常性』の形成に失敗した子どもが、不適応な誇大自己の発達地点で発達停止を起こしている状態と言える。つまり、傲慢な態度や自信過剰な発言は『危険な世界』や『信頼できない他者』に対する防衛機制(過剰防衛)の現れであり、幼児的な全能感を抑制して現実的な野心(理想)を持たせるためには、統合された自己イメージと安定した心理構造の再構築を行う必要がある。自己愛性人格障害を予防する『自己の健全な発達』を実現する為には、『誇大自己の鏡面的自己対象』のプロセスと『理想化された親イマーゴの自己対象の理想化』のプロセスが、共感的に相互作用して、『誇大自己の向上心(野心的願望・自尊心)』を現実的なレベルに調整しなければならない。

一応こんな風になるけれど、だからといって、こんな話を信じるわけでもない。空論だとまではいわない。確かに説得力はあるし、背景には、必然性もある。乳幼児の実際の観察を基盤にしつつもある。精神分析が一般に、その説明の背景として、生物学的な理論と結合しつつあるのはとても意味のあることで、それはまさに、初期フロイト的な方向だと思うし、私はその方向が好きだ。

理解不足を棚に上げて、空理空論と言うつもりもない。こんなことを考える人はいるだろう。しかし、それを支持しそれに賛同する人がこんなに多くなっていることに驚く。

ナルシスティック・パーソナリティは大きな問題だ。そしてもうひとつ、ナルシスティック・パーソナリティをこのような仕方で議論して理論化している人たちがこんなにも多いのだという現状を不思議な思いで眺めている。

同じことはフロイトの前期と後期についてもいえることで、私にとってフロイト前期は実に理由があって、天才的だと思う。ニュートンとライプニッツの登場のように、必然的だったと思う。後期は、あまり感心しない。多分、よく理解していないからだろう。



共通テーマ:健康

この広告は前回の更新から一定期間経過したブログに表示されています。更新すると自動で解除されます。